Posted on Leave a comment

    Cheaper medicines – an unexpected user

    A decorative image with medicine (tablets) red width 700px

    Sometimes you can’t anticipate how your work is going to be used. Here is a story that really motivates me to continue doing what we do.

    To Whom It May Concern,

    It is with sadness that I note your medicine price registry website is no longer working.

    The site was a powerful tool in my medical practice, it really helped me to work out treatments that my patients could afford.

    I’d like to know if the website will be coming back online anytime soon.

    Thank you very much for your efforts in general.

    What a way to find out that someone is using a tool that you built.

    Sometime last year, I developed a simple application that took a little known dataset from the Department of Health website and wrapped it into an easy-to-use format which allowed patients to make sure that they weren’t being overcharged by their pharmacies.

    Through personal use, I realised that the real benefit of the tool was for consumers to find cheaper alternatives to their medicines. My canned elevator pitch when describing what the application does was: “Imagine standing in the queue at the pharmacy waiting to fill your prescription. You take out your cellphone and quickly look up your medicine on this tool and find generic alternatives which are often much cheaper than the branded medicine.”

    I personally saved myself thousands of rands using the tool. This application has been described ad nauseam here and here. The application didn’t get picked up nor did we do much to advertise it.

    Then, during an upgrade of our systems, the website fell over. Sadly, no-one noticed, not even us.

    That was when the email arrived. After further probing, I received this response:

    I’m glad to hear it will be back up soon.

    Actually, one of my own patients let me know about it. It very quickly became a must-use website for me. I work in a mixed-income neighborhoods and being able to figure out what works for my patients’ budgets is extremely helpful – there’s is no point in prescribing medicine that the patient cannot afford to buy.

    Please keep up the good work.

    Wow. It’s not the pat on the back that thrills me, but rather that our work has actually hit the mark. Patients are getting cheaper medicines because of this tool. More than that, it is a use-case that I never expected. I always imagined a patient (probably middle-to-upper middle class) using the tool to save a few bucks. It never occurred to me that a doctor would use it to save money for his patients. In retrospect it is obvious, but certainly not what I expected.

    This tool epitomizes the essence of what OpenUp is all about. Take some complex data. Simplify it, package it and deliver it in a way that an end user would find useful. To make a better, more informed decision. Visit: BrowSearch pronounced Browse & Search the cost of medicines in South Africa, and find Generics. This email has re-ignited my enthusiasm for this tool. The next step is to start marketing it to doctors who can use it for their patients. Thank you OpenUp. Now we need to find your URL. We hope your next project is @Eskom 

    Posted on Leave a comment

    30 of the Most Famous Tales

    from Greek Mythology

    Greek Mythology has left us an invaluable heritage of tales with envious gods, courageous heroes, epic adventures and stories of vengeance and love. The corpus of Greek Mythology is immerse and we would need several volumes of books to cover most of the stories. However, as it is natural, some of those stories are more beloved than others. Here is an abridgment of 30 of the most famous tales from Greek Mythology.

    Image after introtext

    The Infant Zeus Nurtured by the Goat Amalthea, by Nicolas Poussin [Public Domain]

    1. Theogony: Clash of the Titans

    According to HesiodÔÇÖs Theogony, in the beginning, there was only Chaos. Dense darkness covered everything until the Earth was born out of Chaos and the mountains, the sea, and then the sky (Uranus) with the sun, the moon and the stars. Then Uranus and Earth came together and gave birth to the Titans. But, Uranus was afraid that one of his children would take his throne. That is why he enclosed every one of them in the depths of the Earth. But his son, Cronus, the strongest of the Titans, defeated him and became world leader. He married Rhea, who gave birth to two gods and three goddesses: Hades, Poseidon, Hera, Hestia and Demeter.

    But Cronus inherited the fear of his father and believed that one of his offspring would later take his throne. So, when they were born, he swallowed them. However, Rhea was expecting a sixth child and fearing it would share the same fate with her other children, she secretly gave birth on a mountain in Crete and hid the newborn there. She named the child Zeus. She also tricked Cronus into thinking he swallowed this child too, by giving him a stone wrapped in swaddling clothes, which Cronus swallowed thinking it was his newborn. The Nymphs took care of Zeus and fed the baby with the milk of a goat. When he grew up, Zeus found his father and tricked him into drinking a mixture of wine and mustard, which caused him to disgorge the contents of his stomach. ZeusÔÇÖ older brothers and sisters came out of Cronus fully grown! This is how the great Titanomachy began, the war between the Titans and the Gods, with Zeus as their leader. This titanic battle lasted for ten years. The gods defeated the Titans and threw them into Tartarus, a dark and gloomy place as far from the earth as earth is from the sky. Then the gods fought with the Giants for the dominance of the world. The Gigantomachy lasted a long time as well. But the gods were again victorious. Thus, Zeus became the ruler of the whole world and he and the other gods settled in Olympus. 

    The Battle Between the Gods and the Titans

    The Battle Between the Gods and the Titans

    by Joachim Wtewael [Public domain]

    2. ?ñhe Three Sisters of Fate

    In Greek mythology, the Moirae are the three goddesses of fate. Clotho, Lachesis and Atropos. The three sisters weave the fate of humans and gods alike. Neither human nor God has the power to influence or question their judgment and actions! Clotho, the youngest one, spins the thread of life; she is the very origin, the creation of life itself and her thread is spun upon the birth of a person!

    Lachesis, the second sister, is the one that allocates the fate of people during life. The name comes from the Greek word ÔÇÿ??????¤ç?¼?¢¤ëÔÇÖ which means to obtain from lots. In that sense, one can understand that their destiny is chosen out of a myriad of possibilities. It is said that Lachesis measures the thread of life with her rod, determining its length and nature. The last sister of fate is Atropos, the unturning. Atropos is the cutter of the thread of life and with her shears she determines how someone will die.

    'A Golden Thread', by John Melhuish Strudwick

    ‘A Golden Thread’, by John Melhuish Strudwick

    Source: wikipedia.org

    3. Prometheus and the Theft of Fire

    One day, Zeus distributed gifts to all the gods, but he didn’t care much for humans. The Titan Prometheus, however, because he loved and felt sorry for humans, climbed up on Olympus and stole the fire from Hephaestus’ workshop, put it in a hollow reed and gifted it to the humans. This way, humans could create fire, warm up and make tools. Zeus became very angry when he heard about this. He took Prometheus to a high mountain, the Caucasus, and chained him on a rock with thick chains made by the smith god, Hephaestus. And every day, Zeus would send an eagle that ate PrometheusÔÇÖ liver. For thirty years Prometheus remained bound in the Caucasus, until the great hero Hercules, ZeusÔÇÖ demigod son, released him finally from his torment.

    The Punishment of Prometheus

    The Punishment of Prometheus

    by Theodoor Rombouts [Public Domain]

    Related tour image 4

    RELATED TOUR

    Create Your Own Myth in Greece

    LEARN MORE

    4. PandoraÔÇÖs Box

    After Prometheus gave the fire to humans, Zeus decided to take vengeance. He ordered Hephaestus to create the first human woman out of soil and water. Each god gave the woman a gift: Athena gave her wisdom, Aphrodite beauty, Hermes cunning and so on. The name of the woman was Pandora (meaning ÔÇ£all giftsÔÇØ in Greek). Zeus gave Pandora a jar, warning her not to open it under any circumstances and sent her to PrometheusÔÇÖ brother, Epimetheus. Prometheus had warned his brother not to accept any gifts from Zeus. However, Epimetheus accepted Pandora who, although tried hard to resist the temptation, opened the jar and released all evils upon the world. Hatred, war, death, hunger, sickness and all the disasters were immediately released. 

    Pandora's box

    Pandora’s box

    by Charles Edward Perugini [Public domain]

    5. The Abduction of Persephone by Hades

    Persephone was the daughter of Demeter and Zeus. As Persephone grew, so did her beauty. When Hades, the god of the Underworld, saw her, he immediately fell in love with her and decided to abduct her. According to the Homeric Hymn to Demeter, one sunny day the young Persephone was gathering flowers in a field, accompanied by her close friends, the Ocean Nymphs. The carefree Persephone moved away from her companions in search of the most beautiful flower. When she reached out to pluck a wondrous narcissus, Earth yawned open and Hades appeared in his golden chariot and snatched her away to the Underworld while she was in tears. Demeter was looking in vain for her daughter day and night. The land and crops of the earth began to wither. After a while, the Sun, looking at everything from the sky, felt sorry for the goddess and told her what happened. Demeter went to Zeus and demanded that Persephone be returned, or else she would not let the earth blossom again.

    Zeus sent the Messenger God, Hermes, to Hades with the command to release Persephone. Before returning her to Hermes, Hades forced Persephone to eat six pomegranate seeds. Hades knew that if someone ate food in the Underworld, they could never really escape the world of the dead. Persephone was shortly after reunited with her mother. However, Demeter was furious when she heard about the pomegranate seeds. Zeus then proposed a compromise: for every seed Persephone had eaten, she would spend a month with Hades. Demeter accepted ZeusÔÇÖ proposal. Thus, Persephone would travel to the Underworld every six months during which time Demeter would mourn and the earth with her. But after six months, Persephone would return to her and Demeter would be happy again and the earth would blossom once again!

    Hint! this story is part of the mythological explanation of how the Eleusinian Mysteries were founded, the most sacred and secret religious rites of Ancient Greece.

    The Rape of Proserpina

    The Rape of Proserpina

    by Peter Paul Rubens [Public domain]

    Related tour image 1

    RELATED TOUR

    Unlocking the Eleusinian Mysteries TourLEARN MORE

    6. The Name Giving of Athens

    Cecrops, the first king of Attica, had named his city after him, Cecropia. However, the gods of Olympus saw this lovely piece of land and wanted to name it after them and become its patron. The most persistent rivals were Poseidon, the god of the sea, and Athena, the goddess of wisdom. To solve their dispute, Zeus decided that each of them would present a gift to the city and the people of Cecropia would decide which gift was the best, and therefore which god would be the patron of the city. One sunny day, Cecrops and the residents of the city went up to a high hill to watch the gods presenting their gifts. Poseidon was the first to present his gift. He struck a rock with his trident and caused a spring of water to gush forth from the ground. This signified that he was assuring the citizens with water and therefore they wouldnÔÇÖt face any time of drought.

    However, the people were not exactly enchanted with his gift because the water from the spring tasted salty, just like the waters of the sea over which Poseidon ruled. Next, it was the turn of goddess Athena. She struck her spear on the ground and a lovely olive tree jumped out of the earth. The citizens liked this gift better because it would give them food, oil and firewood. This is how Athena became the patroness of the beautiful city and this is how Athens got its name according to Greek Mythology.

    Hint! The hill where the gods presented their gifts was the Acropolis Hill. There is still an olive tree there and some say it is the same tree Athena gifted to the Ancient Athenians. You can discover more exciting stories about Acropolis, in an Acropolis Mythology tour.

    The contest of Athena and Poseidon

    The contest of Athena and Poseidon

    by No?½l Hall?® [Public domain]

    Related tour image 2

    RELATED TOUR

    In the Footsteps of Percy Jackson: Mythology Trip to GreeceLEARN MORE

    7. Theseus and the Minotaur

    In Greek Mythology, Minos’ son, Androgeos, has been ÔÇ£treacherously killedÔÇØ while he was in Athens. Minos immediately sought revenge from the Athenians and as retribution he had them send to Crete several youths every seven or nine years to be devoured by Minotaur, a terrifying monster, half man half bull. The young Athenians were thrown into a dark maze, full of arcades and dead ends, wandering aimlessly, until the Minotaur would find them. Theseus, the son of the king of Athens Aegeus, did not endure this humiliation and demanded to be among the seven young men that were to be sent for the third time to the labyrinth. So, he arrived in Crete and met Minos’ daughter, Ariadne, with whom he fell in love.

    Ariadne then gave Theseus a lame spinner (known as Ariadne’s thread) and advised him to tie his end to the entrance of the labyrinth and unwrap it so that he could find the exit after killing Minotaur. Theseus entered the dark arcades holding the mite and managed to kill the Minotaur by cutting off his head, thus ending Minos’ blood rage. Then he managed to return to the exit, following AriadneÔÇÖs thread. Theseus took Ariadne with him on his boat and began the journey to Athens. However, they made a stop on the island of Naxos, where they celebrated their love. While on the island, the god Dionysus appeared in Theseus’ dream and told him that he had to leave the island without Ariadne since she was meant to stay there and become Dionysus’ wife. Ariadne stayed in Naxos and married Dionysus, while Theseus returned to Athens. The two lovers never met againÔǪ

    Theseus and Ariadne

    Theseus and Ariadne

    by Jean-Baptiste Regnault [Public domain]

    8. Daedalus and Icarus

    The labyrinth in King MinoÔÇÖs palace was designed by a famous inventor and engineer, Daedalus. It is said that Athena herself taught Daedalus. King Minos commissioned to Daedalus and his son Icarus the construction of the labyrinth that would held the monster Minotaur. After finishing their work, King Minos imprisoned father and son inside the labyrinth, in an effort to prevent knowledge of his labyrinth from spreading to the public. Father and son were thinking hard on how to escape until Daedalus came up with an idea. They gathered a lot of feathers from birds and glued them together with wax thus, making four large wings. They tied the wings to each shoulder and fled from the island of Crete. Daedalus had warned Icarus not to fly close to the sun because the wax would melt. After passing the island of Delos, the boy, forgetting himself, flew high towards the sun. The hot sun softened the wax that held the feathers together and Icarus fell in the sea and drowned. Daedalus named the place where his son fell Icaria, in his memory.

    Landscape with the fall of Icarus

    Landscape with the fall of Icarus

    by Joos de Momper [Public Domain]

    9. The Myth of King Aegeus

    The legend has it that before Theseus left for the palace of King Minos in Crete to kill the Minotaur, Aegeus, his father and King of Athens, asked him to change the sails of his ship from black to white on his return home so that he knew that he survived. Aegeus waited patiently in Sounio to see his sonÔÇÖs ship return and the color of its sails. Theseus, although he killed the Minotaur and got out of the labyrinth safely, he forgot to change his sails to white. Seeing the ship with the black sails, Aegeus thought that his beloved son was killed. Sadness and grief filled his heart and mind, and without waiting to get the news from the men on the boats, fell from the rocks of Sounio into the sea below… Since then, the sea is called Aegean in his memory. And his son, Theseus, became the King of Athens.

    Theseus Finds His Father's Sword

    Theseus Finds His Father’s Sword

    by Nicolas Poussin [Public domain]

    10. Perseus and the Gorgon Medusa

    Another famous tale from Greek Mythology is the killing of the terrifying Gorgon Medusa from the great hero Perseus. Perseus was a demigod, son of the Olympian god Zeus and the mortal woman Danae. Perseus sought to kill Medusa, the only mortal of the three monstrous sisters. Instead of hair, Medusa had living venomous snakes on her head and anyone that would look at her eyes would immediately turn into stone. With the help and the wisdom of goddess Athena, Perseus approached the monster by looking through the reflection of a shield and cut off her head.

    Tip! For the “Percy Jackson” fans, Perseus was the inspiration of Rick Riordan (the author of the series) for Percy’s name. If you are visiting Greece, do not miss the opportunity to participate in a Hero’s Quest Inspired by Percy Jackson or customize your very own Percy Jackson-inspired tour, and follow in the footsteps of your beloved heroes.

    Perseus, under the protection of Minerva, turns Phineus to stone by brandishing the head of Medusa

    Perseus, under the protection of Minerva, turns Phineus to stone by brandishing the head of Medusa

    by Jean-Marc Nattier [Public domain]

    You may also like:

    11. The Love Story of Eros and Psyche

    In Greek Mythology, love has the highest praise. Psyche (meaning ÔÇ£soulÔÇØ in Greek), was an impressive mortal girl, surpassing in beauty even the goddess of love, Aphrodite. Her beauty was so well-known that men from all over the land would visit her to admire her beauty. This made Aphrodite extremely jealous and decided to punish the girl. She ordered his son, Eros, who could make someone fall in love by hitting them with his arrows, to make Psyche fall in love with the vilest and despicable creature who walked on Earth. However, when Eros gazed upon Psyche he fell in love with her himself. He could not carry out his motherÔÇÖs order and instead, he remained silent. The years went by and, despite her beauty, Psyche could not marry. All men admired her godly beauty but then would go on and marry another. Her parents decided to go to Delphi and ask for guidance from Apollo. The Oracle said that Psyche had to dress in black, climb a high mountain alone and stay there. Then, a winged serpent would come for her and take her as his wife. Psyche and her parents had no choice but to follow the godÔÇÖs words. As she was waiting alone on the mountain, shaking and crying, the fresh wind of Zephyrus raised her and traveled her through the sky to the gates of a magnificent castle. There, a sweet voice greeted her and made her feel like home.

    “Love cannot live without trust”

    Every night, Eros would come in the dark and lie beside her. Without seeing him, Psyche could feel that he was not a monster but the loving husband she had always been wishing for. The following days passed in full joy and Psyche was happy. However, she missed her family and felt sorry for them. He asked Eros to let her see them and he granted her wish, after warning her not to be influenced by them, otherwise, their relationship will be destroyed and she will suffer a lot. The next day, her two sisters, carried by the wind, arrived to the palace. They felt jealous of her sister living like a goddess and told her that her husband did not allow her to see him because he was the horrible creature the Oracle had mentioned. This idea overwhelmed the mind of Psyche, who could not understand why her husband would not show his face. So, she devised a plan.

    She decided that when Eros falls asleep next to her, she will light a candle to see him. If he is a monster she will kill it with her knife, otherwise, she will happily fall back to sleep. And so she did. But, after seeing his face, a drop of hot oil fell from the candle and woke Eros up. He immediately left her, saying with a heartbroken voice: ÔÇ£Love cannot live without trust.ÔÇØ Psyche was really sorry and sad, and she could not find Eros anywhere. Desperate, she appeared to his mother, goddess Aphrodite, and asked for her help. Aphrodite told her that in order to reunite with her loved one she would have to carry out three impossible tasks. With the help of nature and others, she managed to complete all the tasks and return to Aphrodite. Despite her success, Aphrodite got angry with her and yelled the poor girl that she would never let her go. Witnessing all this, the other gods of Olympus sent Hermes to tell Eros everything that has happened. Eros was touched by PsycheÔÇÖs love and returned to her. From that day on, the couple lived happily together. As a wedding gift, Zeus allowed Psyche to taste the drink of the Gods, Ambrosia, making her immortal. Aphrodite was also happy because now that Psyche was immortal, the men would forget about her and worship once again the true goddess of beauty.

    Cupid and Psyche

    Cupid and Psyche

    by Anthony Van Dyck [public domain]

    12. The Fateful Love of Orpheus and Eurydice

    In Greek Mythology, Orpheus was the greatest lyre player in the world. He could charm rocks and rivers with his music. When Orpheus fell in love with Eurydice, he wooed her with his song. Their marriage was brief, however, as Eurydice was bitten by a viper and died shortly after. Devastated, Orpheus journeyed to the Underworld to convince Hades and Persephone to return his bride to him. Orpheus managed to pass through Cerberus, the three-headed dog who was the guardian of the gates, by making him fall asleep with his music. When he played his lyre, the king and queen of the Underworld were moved by his song, and they agreed to let Eurydice live again on one condition: she would follow him while walking out to the light from the darkness of the Underworld, but he should not turn to look at her before she was out to the light. As they started ascending towards the living world, Orpheus began to think it might all be a trick, that the gods were just making fun of him and Eurydice was not really behind him. Unable to hear Eurydice’s footsteps, Orpheus finally lost his faith and turned to look back, only a few meters away from the exit. Eurydice was in fact behind him, as a shade that would become flesh again when she was back into the light. After Orpheus looked at her, EuridiceÔÇÖs shade fell back into the darkness of the Underworld, now trapped in Hades forever.

    Orpheus and Eurydice

    Orpheus and Eurydice

    by Peter Paul Rubens [Public Domain]

    Related tour image 3

    RELATED TOUR

    Life and Death in Ancient Greece Walking TourLEARN MORE

    13. The Tragic Hero Oedipus

    Laius was the King of Thebes and married to Jocasta. Laius had received an oracle from Delphi saying that his son would kill him and marry his wife. When Jocasta gave birth, Laius tied the babyÔÇÖs ankles and ordered a shepherd-servant to take it to the mountain and abandon it there to die. However, the shepherd took pity on the baby and passed it to another shepherd who gave it to the King of Corinth and his wife, who did not have any children and raised it as their own. They called the child Oedipus, meaning ÔÇ£swollen feetÔÇØ in Greek. When Oedipus grew up, he traveled to Delphi where the Oracle gave him the prophecy that he would kill his own father and marry his mother. Shocked by the words of Apollo, he did not return to Corinth so as to avoid his father and mother. As he was traveling near Thebes, Oedipus met Laius at a crossroads and killed him in a fight, without knowing he was his real father, thus fulfilling the first part of the prophecy.

    When he reached Thebes, he learned of Sphinx, a terrible monster that devoured anyone that did not solve its riddle. It was proclaimed that whoever managed to solve the riddle and kill the Sphinx, he would take the throne of Thebes, by marrying LaiusÔÇÖ widow, Jocasta. Oedipus was successful in solving the riddle and killing the Sphinx. He married Jocasta and together had four children. Little did he know that his children were also his siblings. While Oedipus was at the peak of his happiness, there was an epidemic in Thebes. Oedipus sought the advice of the Oracle of Delphi. The Oracle’s answer was that in order to stop the epidemic, LaiusÔÇÖ killer must be found and punished. The investigation that followed led Oedipus to the truth. Upon realizing the truth, Jocasta, his mother and wife, hanged herself. Oedipus then seized two pins from her dress and blinded himself with them. A Greek tragedy indeedÔǪ

    The Blind Oedipus Commending his Children to the Gods

    The Blind Oedipus Commending his Children to the Gods

    by B?®nigne Gagneraux [Public Domain]

    14. The 12 Labors of Hercules

    Hercules is the most famous hero of Greek Mythology and well-known for his twelve labors. He was a demigod, son of Zeus and Alcmene. Hera, the wife of Zeus, hated Hercules and wanted to kill him. Driven mad by the goddess, Hercules killed his own sons by his wife Megara. After realizing what he did, he traveled to Delphi and asked Apollo how he could atone for his actions. Pythia, the Oracle of Apollo, told him to go to Tiryns and serve his cousin, King Eurystheus, for twelve years. Eurystheus, loathing his cousin, set him to complete twelve impossible labors. He ordered him to: 1) slay the Nemean Lion, 2) slay the nine-headed Lernaean Hydra, 3) capture the Golden Hind of Artemis, 4) capture the Erymanthian Boar, 5) clean the Augean stables in a single day, 6) slay the Stymphalian Birds, 7) capture the Cretan Bull, 8) steal the Mares of Diomedes, 9) obtain the girdle of Hippolyta, Queen of the Amazons, 10) obtain the cattle of the monster Geryon, 11) steal the golden apples of the Hesperides, and 12) capture and bring back Cerberus, the three-headed dog of Hades. Hercules managed to complete all twelve labors and free himself from the service of Eurystheus, having atoned for the killing of his sons. Many more adventures followed until he found a tragic death from his wife, Megara.

    Heracles and the Nemea Lion

    Heracles and the Nemea Lion

    by Pieter Paul Rubens [Public Domain]

    15. The Myth of Apollo and Daphne

    Daphne was a Naiad Nymph in Greek Mythology, the daughter of a river god. She was famous for being incredibly beautiful and for catching the eye of god Apollo. However, Daphne was determined to remain unmarried and untouched by a man for the rest of her life. According to Greek Mythology, Apollo had been mocking the God of Love, Eros. In retaliation, Eros fired two arrows: a golden arrow that struck Apollo and made him madly in love with Daphne, and a lead arrow that made Daphne hate Apollo.

    Under the spell of the arrow, Apollo continued to chase Daphne, but she continued to reject him. Apollo told Daphne that he would love her forever. Daphne turned to the river god, Peneus, and pleaded to him to free her from Apollo. In response, Peneus used metamorphosis to turn Daphne into a laurel tree. Apollo used his powers of eternal youth and immortality to make Daphnes laurel leaves evergreen. It is believed that Daphne had to sacrifice her body and turn into a tree, as this was the only way she could avoid Apollos sexual advances. After Daphne had been transformed into a laurel, Apollo made the plant sacred and vowed to always wear it as clothing. Thus, in a way, Daphne stayed with Apollo forever

    Apollo and Daphne

    Apollo and Daphne

    by Nicolas Poussin [Public Domain]

    16. The One-sided Love Story of Pan and Syrinx

    Pan was the god of fertility and the patron of shepherds and huntsmen in Greek Mythology; he presided over all rural occupations, he was chief of the Satyrs and head of all rural divinities. According to the common belief, he was the son of Hermes and a wood nymph, and came into the world with horns sprouting from his forehead, a goat’s beard and a crooked nose, pointed ears, and the tail and feet of a goat. He had such a repulsive appearance that, at the sight of him, his mother fled in dismay. Hermes, however, took up his curious little offspring, wrapped him in a hare skin, and carried him in his arms to Olympus. The grotesque form and the merry antics of the little Pan made him a great favorite with all the immortals, especially Dionysus; and they bestowed upon him the name of Pan (meaning ÔÇ£allÔÇØ in Greek) because he had delighted them all. PanÔÇÖs life was defined by his relationships with the Nymphs. He loved them deeply, he was dancing and playing music with them, and some of them loved him too; others hated him and were running away from him… very complicated relationships indeed… And his mother issues appeared soon enough.

    The spirit of the reed tree comes from a nymph. Her name was Syrinx. Pan was the one to cause her doom. He was in love with her and wanted her at any cost! He was chasing after her trying to make her his! So, in order to escape him, she transformed herself into a reed tree. She hid by the river among the other reeds but Pan would not stop there. He went down to the river and started ripping off every reed until he finally found her. He ripped her off the ground and started blowing into the pipes to get her spirit out. While he was blowing, he realized that beautiful sounds were coming out of the reed pipes. He decided to bind them together into a big flute and started making music out of them. Oh, and what beautiful music he made! From then on, he would never leave his flute and he would always play for the other nymphs to dance

    Pan and Syrinx

    Pan and Syrinx

    by Adam Elsheimer [Public domain]

    17. Goddess Athena and Arachne

    In Greek Mythology Gods were powerful and humans should be obedient. But was that always the case? In ancient times there was a beautiful lady called Arachne (meaning ÔÇ£spiderÔÇØ in Greek). She knew the art of loom very well and she weaved beautifully. She boasted that she could weave better than Athena, who was the patroness of the weaving art. She even dared to ask the goddess to a contest. Athena accepted and they began to weave. Athena weaved a representation of her fight with Poseidon over the naming of Athena. Arachne, on the other hand, weaved the naughty adventures of Zeus and the other gods of Olympus. Athena, angered by the hubris Arachne dared to show, transformed her into a spider and cursed her to be hanging from her web for the remainder of her life.

    Pallas and Arachne

    Pallas and Arachne

    by Peter Paul Rubens [FAL]

    18. The Myth of Narcissus and Echo

    Echo was a wood Nymph, cursed by Hera to not be able to speak properly, but rather repeat the last words addressed to her. One day, she was wandering around the mountains, until she saw a handsome young man that no one could resist his charm, Narcissus. The Nymph fell in love with the youth, but could not speak to him because of HeraÔÇÖs curse. So, she was following him from the shadows, silently and in love, waiting patiently for the proper moment. At some point, Narcissus felt her presence and asked ÔÇ£Is anybody here?ÔÇØ, to which Echo replied ÔÇ£hereÔÇØ. A confusing and repetitive conversation followed until Narcissus called her to come out and make love with him. But, as soon as Echo stepped out, Narcissus told her that heÔÇÖd rather die than give himself to a wood nymph.

    Echo, heartbroken, took refuge in a cave and lost her appetite for food or water. After a while, poor Echo started growing skinny from starvation until her body disappeared, living only dust and her voice. Nemesis, the goddess of revenge against those who show hubris, decided to punish Narcissus for the treatment of poor Echo. The goddess made Narcissus fall in love with his own reflection that he saw in a pond near EchoÔÇÖs cave. Narcissus could not leave his own reflection out of love and starved to death, like Echo. But, before he dies, Narcissus cried out to his reflection ÔÇ£Farewell, dear boy. Beloved in vain.ÔÇØ EchoÔÇÖs voice repeated his last words from the cave as Narcissus drew his last breath. To this day, Echo still repeats the last words or phrases in caves or labyrinths.

    Echo and Narcissus

    Echo and Narcissus

    by John William Waterhouse [Public Domain]

    19. The Myth of Hermaphroditus

    Hermaphroditus was the son of Hermes and Aphrodite, and he was raised by nymphs in the caves of Mount Phrygia. On his face, one could see the grace and beauty of both of his parents, from whom he took his name. When he was fifteen years old, he left the mountain where he grew up to wander into Asia Minor and meet new people. In the woods of Caria, he stopped to rest and drink water from a spring called Salmacis. The homonymous nymph, Salmacis, was captivated by the beauty of the young man and tried to seduce him, but was rejected. When Hermaphroditus felt he was alone, he jumped into the water naked to swim. Salmacis appeared behind a tree and jumped in as well, wrapping her body around that of the young man, forcibly kissing and touching him. While Hermaphroditus was trying to break free from her, the nymph called out to the gods to let them be united forever. The gods decided to grant her wish and blended their two bodies into one, creating a creature of both sexes. Hermaphroditus prayed to his parents, Hermes and Aphrodite, that anyone else who bathed in that spring would share his fate. And the gods granted his wish.

    Wooded Pool with Salmacis and Hermaphroditus

    Wooded Pool with Salmacis and Hermaphroditus

    by Moyses van Wtenbrouck [Public domain]

    20. Bellerophon and Pegasus

    Bellerophon was a great hero of Greek Mythology. He was a slayer of monsters and as famous as Perseus. His most famous feat was the killing of Chimera, a horrible monster with a goat’s body, a serpent’s tail and a lion’s head that could breathe fire. The great hero was the son of the mortal woman Eurynome by either her husband and King of Corinth Glaucus or the god Poseidon. Bellerophon was accompanied in his adventures by the winged horse, Pegasus. The Myth has it that Pegasus was born from the blood of her beheaded mother Medusa, the gorgon who was tricked and killed by the hero Perseus. A more detailed version of the myth said that he was born when MedusaÔÇÖs blood was mixed with the foam of the sea. The myth says that Pegasus was born as a winged horse because his father, Poseidon, had the shape of a horse when he seduced Medusa. When Pegasus was born, a huge thunder with lightning pierced the sky, and thatÔÇÖs how his connection to the forces of the skies was established.

    So, one day Bellerophon saw the wonderful animal drinking water and decided to tame it. This would not have been possible if Athena had not helped him, by giving him a golden bridle. After slaying Chimera, Bellerophon’s fame grew and so did his arrogance. He believed that because of his great feat, he deserved to fly to the mountain of the gods, Olympus, and set course with Pegasus. Zeus was angered by his hubris and sent a gadfly to sting Pegasus, causing Bellerophon to fall off the flying horse. Pegasus continued to Olympus and became the loyal horse of Zeus. It is said that Athena spared the life of Bellerophon by softening the ground for his fall. However, the once-great hero spent his remaining life crippled and lonely, always searching for his beloved winged horse.

    Bellerophon on Pegasus

    Bellerophon on Pegasus

    by Giovanni Battista Tiepolo [Public domain]

    21. Leda and the Swan

    Another tale from Greek mythology about Zeus is the one with Leda. When the Olympian god saw Leda on the banks of the river Eurotas, felt an overwhelming desire for her. So, he went to Aphrodite and asked for her advice. Aphrodite transformed Zeus into a brilliant swan and herself into an eagle, and she began pursuing the swan in the river valley. The pursuing Zeus swan sought refuge in the arms of Leda, who received him tenderly and warmed him within her. However, nine months after this incident, she gave birth to two eggs. Not one swan came out of each, but two pairs of twins. On one hand Polydefkis and the beautiful Helen and on the other Castor and Klytemnestra! Her kids became very famous and lead characters to many great ancient tragedies!

    Leda and the Swan

    Leda and the Swan

    by Fran?ºois Boucher [Public domain]

    22. The Myth of Andromeda and Perseus

    Long ago, the African Kingdom of Ethiopia was ruled by a king named Cepheus and his queen Cassiopeia. The royal couple had a daughter, Andromeda. One day, the queen boasted about her beauty before the mythical inhabitants of the sea – the Nereids. Nereids became very angry because they believed themselves the most beautiful creatures in the world. They complained to their father, the god of the seas, Poseidon, that he should punish her. The mighty lord of the seas sent a huge sea monster to ravage the coast of Ethiopia, including the vain queen. The desperate King asked for the guidance of Apollo in Delphi. The Oracle advised that no respite would be found until the king and the queen sacrificed their daughter to the monster. To appease Poseidon, Cepheus and Cassiopeia offered, reluctantly, their beloved daughter to be eaten by the monster. The beautiful Andromeda was chained on a seaside rock and awaited her fate.

    Meanwhile, the great hero Perseus was returning from having slain Medusa, riding the winged horse, Pegasus, high in the sky. Passing over Ethiopia, he saw Andromeda chained on the rock and immediately fell in love with her. As the monster was approaching to devour Andromeda, the brave Perseus started fighting it; their fight lasted for a long time. Finally, Perseus used the deadly look of Medusa’s severed head, petrifying the huge monster which fell in the sea and became an island. Perseus freed Andromeda from her chains and took her back to the palace of Cepheus where they got married. They lived together happily, having seven sons and two daughters. After AndromedaÔÇÖs death, goddess Athena placed her among the constellations in the northern sky, near Perseus and Cassiopeia. 

    Metamorphoses of Ovide

    Metamorphoses of Ovide

    by Pierre Mignard [ Public Domain]

    Related tour image 4

    RELATED TOUR

    Create Your Own Myth in Greece

    LEARN MORE

    23. The Myth of Sisyphus and his Eternal Punishment

    Once upon a time, Corinth was a very strong Greek city-state, the remains of which can be found to this day. Some sources refer to the great city of Efyra as the city founded by Sisyphus, which was later named Corinth. Others say that the witch Medea gave Corinth to Sisyphus, who became its king. One day, Asopos’ daughter, Aegina, had been abducted by Zeus and when Asopos asked if Sisyphus had seen anything, Sisyphus mentioned that he saw Zeus fly over with Aegina. When Zeus heard that, he got really angry that he was betrayed by a mortal. So, the king of the gods sent Death to take Sisyphus’ life. However, when Death came to chain Sisyphus, the latter asked Death a demonstration of how the chains work and then deceived Death and chained him instead.

    The imprisonment of Death meant that he could not come for any human and people stopped dying. The gods in response sent Ares, the god of war, to free Death. This time Death took Sisyphus in his chains and led him to the world of the dead, the Underworld, kingdom of Hades. However, before he died, Sisyphus asked his wife, Merope, not to bury him properly by neglecting to put a coin in his mouth. This way he could not pay Charon, the ferryman, to cross the river Styx. The lack of a proper burial disturbed Hades so much, that he sent Sisyphus back to the living. Thus, Sisyphus managed to escape Death once more. When the gods finally managed to catch Sisyphus again, they decided that his punishment should last forever. They made him push a rock up a mountain; every time the rock would reach the top, it would roll down again and Sisyphus would have to start all over again.

    Sisyphus

    Sisyphus

    by Franz Stuck [Public domain]

    24. King Midas and his Golden Touch

    In Greek Mythology, Midas was the king of Phrygia and ruled from his castle and its beautiful garden in which ÔÇ£roses grow of themselves, each bearing sixty blossoms and of surpassing fragranceÔÇØ, according to Herodotus. One day, some of MidasÔÇÖ people found a drunken old man near the garden and brought him before the king. Midas recognized the old man, who was god DionysusÔÇÖ closest reveler, the satyr Silenus. Instead of punishing him, Midas hosted the satyr for ten days, offering him food, drinks and entertaining him. When he returned him safely to Dionysus, the god felt gratitude and offered Midas to grant him any wish he had. Midas, motivated by his greed, asked that he should be able to turn into gold everything he touched. At first, Midas gained great wealth and power from his unique ability. But he later realized that it was more of a curse than a gift. Even the water and the food that he touched was turning into gold. He could not enjoy even the simplest joys in life anymore. Midas went back to Dionysus and begged him to take back his power.

    King Midas turns an oak branch to gold

    King Midas turns an oak branch to gold

    by Nicolas Poussin [Public Domain]

    25. The Apple of Discord

    The great Trojan War started with a few envious Gods and an apple… During the wedding of Thetis and Peleus, the goddess of discord, Eris, was not invited for apparent reasons. Eris felt offended and, arriving at the wedding, tossed in the middle of the feast of the gods a golden apple, saying ÔÇ£to the fairestÔÇØ. The apple was claimed by Hera, Athena and Aphrodite, sparking a vanity-fueled dispute among the three. The goddesses asked Zeus who the apple belonged to (in other words, who is the fairest of them all) and Zeus said that Paris, a mortal man and the rightful Prince of Troy, should choose.

    Paris at the time was living as a shepherd on Mount Ida and was not aware of his royal descent. He had been abandoned as a baby, because of an oracle that said he would cause the destruction of his city. The three goddesses appeared before the shepherd Paris and asked him to choose who is the fairest of them all. Because Paris at first was unable to choose one, each of the goddesses offered him a gift: Hera offered him wealth and kingly power, Athena wisdom and glory among men, and Aphrodite offered him the love of the most beautiful woman in the world. Without hesitation, Paris gave the golden apple to Aphrodite. From that day on, Aphrodite was offering council to Paris. She was the one that informed him of his royal blood and led him back to Troy. The rest is history

    Golden Apple of Discord

    Golden Apple of Discord

    by Jacob Jordaens [Public domain]

    26. The Great Trojan War

    The events that occurred in the myth of the Apple of Discord would lead to the greatest war of Greek Mythology. The Trojan War is an epic poem, written by Homer. Having been promised by Aphrodite the love of the most beautiful woman, Paris abducted Helen, wife of Menelaus of Sparta. Refusing to return Helen, MenelausÔÇÖ brother, Agamemnon, gathered a great army of Greeks to sail to Troy. At Aulis, the army was gathered, with the greatest Greek heroes among them – Achilles, Patroclus, Odysseus, Nestor to name a few. However, there was no wind for the ships to sail and the warriors started to complain. The reason for this was the killing of ArtemisÔÇÖ sacred deer by Agamemnon. The Greek King was forced to sacrifice his daughter, Iphigenia, to appease Artemis and the winds came.

    For nine years the Greek army was trying to enter the walls of Troy without any luck. Finally, Odysseus had an idea to build a gigantic hollow wooden horse, in which a small group of warriors would conceal. The other Greeks appeared to sail for home, leaving behind the horse as a gift to the Trojans. Despite the warnings of Cassandra and others, the Trojans took the horse inside the walls and celebrated with a lot of wine and music. When everyone was asleep, the Greek warriors crept out of the horse and opened the gates. The Greek army entered without resistance and Troy fell. Achilles died during the battle, having been hit in the heel by an arrow. The gods also took part in the war. Hera, Poseidon and Athena aided the Greeks, while Ares and Aphrodite the Trojans. 

    The Abduction of Helen

    The Abduction of Helen

    by Francesco Primaticcio [Public domain]

    27. The Legendary Myth of Odysseus

    Odysseus (also known with his Latin name ÔÇÿUlyssesÔÇÖ) was a great hero of HomerÔÇÖs epic poems Iliad and Odyssey. The Odyssey recounts his adventures since he left Troy, in his effort to return home. His wandering lasted for no less than ten years! His adventures were many: he fought against the Cicones, broke free from the Lotus-Eaters, escaped with cunning the Cyclop Polyphemus and son of Poseidon by blinding him, making the sea god his enemy. He then visited the island of Aelous, the Wind God, receiving a sack as a gift, which contained all the winds inside, to help him arrive home. As they were arriving in Ithaca, two of his men opened the sack out of curiosity while Odysseus was sleeping and their ship was once again away from Ithaca because of the storm that followed. He then survived the Laestrygonians, a tribe of man-eating giants and landed on the island of the sorceress Circe. With the help of Hermes, Odysseus left the island and journeyed to the Underworld, to get help from the blind prophet Tiresias who had died. He then passed through the Sirens and their seductive song by blocking the ears of his men with wax and ordering them to tie him up to the mast, so that he could not jump and join the Sirens. His next challenge was to cross the strait between Scylla, a six-headed monster, and Charybdis, a violent whirlpool, which he managed to do by sacrificing six of his men.

    He lost the remaining of his men and his ship at the island of Thrinacia, after Zeus threw a thunderbolt to appease the sun god Helios. Odysseus found himself next to the island of Ogygia, where he spent seven years with the goddess Calypso who had fallen in love with him. With the help of Hermes, he left the island with a raft he made. A storm washed him this time at the island of the Phaeacians. This time he was lucky since the island was protected by King Alcinous and his Queen Arete, who helped him return to Ithaca. When he finally arrived, twenty years after setting sail for Troy, he found that his palace was inhabited by young people from noble families in the surrounding islands and Ithaca. Each of them wanted to marry Penelope, his wife, because they believed Odysseus did not survive. Penelope patiently waited all these years for the return of her husband, devising a trick to delay her suitors. Odysseus killed them all with his bow, with the help of his son Telemachus and his faithful dog. But as soon as he killed the suitors, their fathers got angry and demanded revenge. Finally, goddess Athena, his everlasting protector, brought peace to the island and Odysseus and his wife Penelope were reunited and happy at last.

    Ulysses and the Sirens

    Ulysses and the Sirens

    by Herbert James Draper [Public domain]

    28. The Adventures of Jason and the Argonauts

    One of the most famous stories of Greek Mythology is that of Jason and the Argonauts, and their quest for the Golden Fleece. Jason was the son of Aeson, rightful heir to the throne of Iolcus. Pelias, the half-brother of Aeson, took the throne of Iolcus, bypassing Aeson and locking him in the dungeons. Pelias received an oracle from Delphi that a descendant of Aeson would seek revenge. Pelias believed that Jason was the one that the Oracle meant, so he sent him to undertake an impossible mission, hoping that he will be slain in the process. The mission was to retrieve the Golden Fleece from the land of Colchis. The Golden Fleece was the skin of a winged holy ram of god Zeus and it was guarded by a huge dragon. For the great adventure, Jason assembled the best heroes of Greece, including Hercules and Orpheus, and had a special boat built, named Argos. So, Jason and the Argonauts began their journey. After a challenging voyage, they arrived at Colchis and asked the Golden Fleece from King Aeetes. The King deceived Jason and put him into great danger, only for Medea, AeetesÔÇÖ daughter, to save him. Medea was a sorceress and fell in love with Jason.

    She told him that she would help him retrieve the Fleece if he would then take her back with him and marry her. Jason agreed and Medea put a spell on the dragon, allowing Jason to retrieve the Fleece. Jason and the argonauts, together with Medea, returned to Argos and set sail away from Colchis. However, before they leave, Medea killed her brother, spreading his pieces across the ocean, so that her father would not follow them before he gathers all the pieces. Zeus was angry with the killing of MedeaÔÇÖs brother and sent many trials to the Argonauts. They had to pass through the Sirens, the Skylla and Charybdis, Talos and many more. By overcoming all these obstacles, the Argonauts redeemed themselves and managed to return back home and give the Golden Fleece to King Pelias. Jason kept his promise to Medea and married her. With her help, they killed Pelias and had two children together. Tragedy, of course, could not be absent from this story either. Jason fell in love with Glaucus and, full of revenge and madness, Medea killed their two children, fleeing to Athens. Jason fell into despair. He returned to his, rotten now, ship, Argos and sat on the sand under it. One piece from the rotten ship peeled off and killed him. 

    The Golden Fleece

    The Golden Fleece

    by Herbert Draper [Public Domain]

    29. The Myth of Leto

    Leto was a female Titan and a favorite lover of Zeus in his early days. While she was pregnant with ZeusÔÇÖ children, Zeus married goddess Hera. As expected, Hera was furious and very jealous of Leto for bearing her husbandÔÇÖs children. She did everything in her power to make the life of Leto difficult and tried her best not to allow her to give birth to ZeusÔÇÖ children. She pushed Leto out of Olympus. While Leto was wandering on Earth, no man would open his house for her, fearing the wrath of Hera. On top of that, Hera had the huge serpent Python to chase her. Zeus saved Leto by sending the North Wind, Boreas, to carry her out to the sea. Finally, a desolate, rocky island named Delos accepted her, having nothing to lose. Leto gave birth first to Artemis and nine days later to Apollo. The children would later grow up to become powerful gods and members of the Greek Pantheon. Trained by their mother, they became very skilled archers. However, HeraÔÇÖs vengeance did not end there. She continued tormenting Leto, having Python chase her everywhere. Finally, only four years old, Apollo killed Python in Delphi.

    Latona and the Lycian Peasants

    Latona and the Lycian Peasants

    by Joshua Cristall [Public Domain]

    30. The Myth of Niobe

    This story is connected to the myth of Leto. Niobe in Greek Mythology was the daughter of Tantalus and Dione or Euryanassa. She was married to Amphion and had fourteen children in total, seven boys and seven girls. She boasted about the fact that she was blessed with so many children and made fun of Leto that she only had two, Apollo and Artemis. For her hubris, Leto punished Niobe by sending Apollo to kill with his arrows all of NiobeÔÇÖs boys and Artemis to kill all of her girls. Upon seeing her dead children, Niobe, in despair, fled to Mount Sipylus where she turned into a rock. The rock became known as the ÔÇÿWeeping RockÔÇÖ.

    Hint! the Weeping Rock resembles truly a woman figure and can still be visited in todayÔÇÖs Turkey.

    Apollo and Diana Attacking the Children of Niobe

    Apollo and Diana Attacking the Children of Niobe

    by Jacques-Louis David [Public Domain]

    Discover more tales from Greek Mythology while walking at the places where the events took place. Join one of our Mythology tours, or send us a tailor-made request to create a tour based on the mythological story you love the most. If you are a fan of Percy Jackson series, make sure to take a mythology trip to Greece in the footsteps of Percy Jackson. Our team of local experts and storytellers will make sure you will live your own myth in Greece!

    More like this:

    Posted on Leave a comment

    Prescription stimulants in individuals with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: misuse, cognitive impact, and adverse effects

    Shaheen E. Lakhan & Annette Kirchgessner
    Global Neuroscience Initiative Foundation, Los Angeles, California

    disorder, oppositional-de´¼üant disorder, antisocial person-
    ality, substance use, and anxiety) (Faraone et al. 1997;
    Rasmussen and Gillberg 2000; Kollins et al. 2005; Bieder-
    man et al. 2006). However, early treatment may decrease
    negative outcomes of ADHD including the rate of con-
    duct disorder and adult antisocial personality disorder
    (Dopheide and Pliszka 2009).
    There are both pharmacological and nonpharmacologi-
    cal (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy [CBT]) treatments
    of ADHD. Stimulants, such as methylphenidate (MPH;
    Ritalin and Concerta) and dextroamphetamine-AMP
    (d-AMP; Adderall) are the most common pharmacologic
    treatments (The MTA Cooperative Group 1999) and
    abundant data support the potentially positive effects of
    prescription stimulants for the majority of children, ado-
    lescents, and adults with ADHD. Experts estimate that
    approximately 60% of children with ADHD are treated
    with prescription stimulants (Center for Disease Control
    and Prevention 2005a); therefore, approximately three
    million children in this country take stimulants for prob-
    lems with focusing. At the same time, many studies have
    revealed the numerous adverse effects associated with pre-
    scription stimulants when they are used inappropriately.
    Stimulants are classi´¼üed as Schedule II drugs (i.e., pro-
    viding positive medicinal effects but also considerable
    abuse potential). The nonmedical use of prescription
    stimulants represents the second common most form of
    illicit drug use in college, second only to marijuana use
    (Johnston et al. 2004). Indeed, many consider stimulants
    ÔÇô whether obtained by prescription or illicitly ÔÇô a conve-
    nient option to improve performance or to induce
    euphoria (get ÔÇ£highÔÇØ). Major daily newspapers such as
    The New York Times have reported a trend toward grow-
    ing use of prescription stimulants, commonly called
    ÔÇ£smart pills,ÔÇØ by high school and college students for
    enhancing school or work performance (Jacobs 2005).
    Unfortunately, media reports appear to condone this
    behavior as 95% of articles mentioned at least one possi-
    ble bene´¼üt of using a prescription stimulant for neuroen-
    hancement, but only 58% mentioned any risks/side effects
    (Partridge et al. 2011). Stimulant misuse is often pre-
    dicted on individualsÔÇÖ misconceptions or simple lack of
    knowledge of associated risks.
    This review discusses recent studies regarding the use
    and misuse of stimulants among high school and college
    students, including athletes, with and without ADHD.
    Given the widespread belief that prescription stimulants
    are ÔÇ£smart pills,ÔÇØ we address if these drugs actually
    enhance cognition in a healthy individual. Athletes may
    see stimulants as a way to help maintain physical ´¼ütness
    for their competitive sport or to improve their concentra-
    tion. Finally, we elaborate on the long-term effects of
    chronic stimulant use. Addiction and tolerance are major
    concerns, as are psychosis and cardiovascular effects. Sur-
    prisingly, these associated risks of stimulant misuse are
    not frequently addressed in the media and literature.
    Clearly, the widespread misuse of prescription stimulants
    represents an important public health issue faced by stu-
    dents, school of´¼ücials, health centers, and parents.
    Methods
    This review was initiated with a PubMed search of the
    US National Library of Medicine with combinations of
    the following key words: ÔÇ£Adderall,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£amphetamine,ÔÇØ
    ÔÇ£methylphenidate,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£dexamphetamine,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£ADHD,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£mis-
    use,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£illicit use,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£non-prescription use,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£non-medical
    use,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£diversion,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£students,ÔÇØ and ÔÇ£athletes.ÔÇØ A review of
    all titles was conducted to include only pertinent publica-
    tions. A hand search of psychiatry journals was performed
    and reference lists from relevant studies were searched.
    Prescription stimulant use in ADHD
    It is estimated that about two-thirds of the children diag-
    nosed with ADHD receive pharmacological treatment
    (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2010) and
    the majority of medications used are stimulants (Center
    for Disease Control and Prevention 2005b). The pre-
    scribed use of stimulant medications to treat ADHD in
    children age 18 and younger rose steadily from 1996 to
    2008, from an estimated 2.4% in 1996 to an estimated
    3.5% of US children in 2008 (Zuvekas and Vitiello 2011).
    Overall, prescription stimulant use among 6- to 12-year-
    olds is highest, going from 4.2% in 1996 to 5.1% in 2008;
    however, the fastest growth rate occurred among
    13ÔÇô18 year olds, going from 2.3% in 1996 to 4% in 2008.
    Prescription stimulant use remained consistently low in
    the West than in other US regions and in lower racial/
    ethnic minorities.
    MPH and d-AMP are the most widely used prescrip-
    tion stimulants approved by the US Food and Drug
    Administration (FDA) for the treatment of ADHD. MPH
    is a short-acting stimulant drug. Generic MPH is available
    in many forms, and several versions of the long-acting
    MPH have been introduced, with Concerta getting the
    largest share of the market. According to the U.S. Drug
    Enforcement Administration (DEA), MPH has been the
    fourth most prescribed controlled substance in the United
    States since 2003, with over 58,000 Americans purchasing
    MPH in 2006 (Department of Justice: Drug Enforcement
    Administration 2008). Both the production and prescrip-
    tion of MPH has risen as the diagnosis of ADHD has
    concurrently increased. In addition, with the realization
    that ADHD is a lifelong disorder, MPH has become more
    commonly prescribed for adolescents and adults, and
    662 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessnertreatment duration has increased (Horrigan 2001). Both
    MPH and d-AMP are ef´¼ücacious and well-tolerated medi-
    cations and remain the ´¼ürst choice for short duration
    management in adolescent and adult ADHD (Faraone
    and Glatt 2010). Although the precise mechanisms under-
    lying the action of these medications are not completely
    understood, they appear to increase the availability of
    dopamine, which could account for their therapeutic
    effects.
    Although ADHD is a multifactorial disorder, disrupted
    dopamine (DA) neurotransmission plays an important
    role in its pathophysiology. In addition, polymorphisms
    in the dopamine D1 receptor (DRD1) are associated with
    the disorder (Misener et al. 2004). MPH and d-AMP both
    enhance DA signaling in the brain. MPH increases DA by
    blocking dopamine transporters (DATs) and AMP by
    releasing DA from the nerve terminal using the DAT as
    carrier (Kuczenski and Segal 1997). In healthy controls
    and in adolescents and adults with ADHD (Rosa-Neto
    et al. 2005; Volkow et al. 2007), MPH signi´¼ücantly
    increased DA in the ventral striatum (VS) (Volkow et al.
    2012), a crucial brain region involved with motivation
    and reward (Wise 2002). Moreover, intravenous MPH-
    induced increases in DA in the VS were correlated with
    improvement in symptoms of inattention after long-term
    oral MPH treatment. Historically, the core feature of
    ADHD has been characterized as one of attention de´¼ücit,
    but increasing evidence suggests that a reward and moti-
    vation de´¼ücit may be of equal importance. It has been
    proposed that increasing DA in the VS would enhance
    the saliency of the task, thus improving attention in
    ADHD (Volkow et al. 2012). Intravenous MPH also sig-
    ni´¼ücantly increased DA in the prefrontal and temporal
    cortices that were associated with decreased ratings of
    inattention, which may be therapeutically relevant.
    The widespread use of prescription stimulants for
    ADHD has not been without critics. In recent months,
    we have heard speculation about whether ADHD is a real
    disease, and if it is real, whether it is being grossly over-
    diagnosed. Disorders often become widely diagnosed after
    drugs come along that can alter a set of suboptimal
    behaviors. In this way, Ritalin and Adderall helped make
    ADHD a household name. If there is a pill that can clear
    up the wavering focus of sleep-deprived youth, then those
    rather ordinary states may come to be seen as syndrome.
    A recent opinion piece entitled ÔÇ£Ritalin Gone WrongÔÇØ in
    the New York Times (Sroufe 2012) by psychology profes-
    sor L. Alan Sroufe argues that attention-de´¼ücit drugs do
    more harm than good over the long term, a conclusion
    other professionals in his ´¼üeld dispute. Studies have
    shown that children who take MPH can show reductions
    in ADHD symptomatology (inattention, hyperactivity,
    and impulsivity) and gains in social and classroom
    behaviors. Studies of adults with ADHD have con´¼ürmed
    its usefulness for this population as well. However, the
    bene´¼üts of prescription stimulants on ADHD symptom-
    atology do not appear to last long.
    The Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with
    ADHD (MTA) compared four distinct treatment strate-
    gies during childhood for children diagnosed with DSM-
    IV ADHD, Combined Type (The MTA Cooperative
    Group 1999). Children were randomly assigned to
    14 months of (a) systematic medication management
    (MedMgt), which was initial placebo-controlled titration,
    three times a day dosing, 7 days a week, and monthly
    30-min clinic visits, (b) multicomponent behavior therapy
    (Beh), which included 27-session group parent training
    supplemented with eight individual parent sessions, an
    8-week summer treatment program, 12 weeks of
    classroom administered behavior therapy with a half-time
    aide, and ten teacher consultation sessions, (c) their
    combination (Comb), or (d) usual community care (CC).
    This randomized, six-site, controlled clinical trial featured
    rigorous diagnostic criteria at study entry and compared
    the relative effectiveness of treatments of well-established
    ef´¼ücacy. The initial MTA ´¼ündings reported that all groups
    showed improvement over baseline at the end of the
    14-month treatment period; however, the Comb and
    MedMgt group participants showed signi´¼ücantly greater
    improvements in ADHD symptoms than did the Beh or
    CC participants. By the next follow-up, 3 years after
    enrollment, there were no longer signi´¼ücant treatment
    group differences in ADHD symptoms or functioning
    (Jensen et al. 2007). Molina et al. (Molina et al. 2009)
    reported the next two follow-up assessments of the MTA
    sample at 6 and 8 years after random assignment, when
    the sample ranged in age from 13 to 18 years and found
    similar ´¼ündings.
    Prevalence of prescription stimulant
    misuse
    The misuse of a stimulant medication ÔÇô taking a stimu-
    lant not prescribed by a physician or in a manner not in
    accordance with physician guidance ÔÇô has been growing
    over the past two decades. In fact, in the past 10 years
    there has been a surge in prevalence rates of non-
    prescription stimulant use among both adolescents and
    young adults. In general, nonprescription use of MPH in
    2000 was reported as 1.2% and in 2006 this number had
    risen to 2%. Breaking the sample down by age, nonpre-
    scription use among adolescents (ages 12ÔÇô17) went from
    2.2% to 1.8% between 2000 and 2006, a slight decrease.
    Among college-aged individuals (ages 18ÔÇô25), however,
    usage increased signi´¼ücantly from 3.6% in 2000 to 5.4%
    by 2006. Finally, among those 26 and older, usage is the
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 663
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDlowest of any group, but rates are rising. In 2000, only
    0.7% reported any lifetime usage of MPH, but this num-
    ber had doubled to 1.5% by 2006 (Bogle and Smith
    2009).
    The majority of research on the misuse of prescription
    stimulants has focused on undergraduate college students.
    The nonprescription use of stimulants has increased in
    this population, to the extent that the misuse of prescrip-
    tion stimulants is second only to marijuana as the most
    common form of illicit drug use among college students
    (Johnston et al. 2004). A 2001 nationwide self-reported
    survey of more than 10,000 students from 4-year univer-
    sities in the United States reported a 6.9% lifetime preva-
    lence of nonprescription stimulant misuse, including a
    past-year prevalence of 4.1% and a past-month prevalence
    of 2.1% (McCabe et al. 2005). Colleges with the highest
    past-year prevalence rates were typically located in the
    northeastern United States, which is corroborated by
    other reports (McCabe et al. 2005). A study by Teter
    et al. (2005) of 9161 undergraduates reported an 8.1%
    lifetime nonprescription stimulant misuse rate among col-
    lege students, including 5.4% over the past year. Accord-
    ing to a 2002 survey of a single US college, 35.5% of
    undergraduates reported using stimulants without a pre-
    scription, with greater frequency occurring in males com-
    pared with females (Low and Gendaszek 2002).
    The majority of nonprescription stimulant users
    reported obtaining the drugs from a peer with a prescrip-
    tion ÔÇô a process termed diversion. The diversion of stim-
    ulants is very common and can begin in childhood,
    adolescence, or young adulthood. A study conducted by
    Wilens et al. (2008) reported that lifetime rates of diver-
    sion ranged from 16% to 29% of students with stimulant
    prescriptions asked to give, sell, or trade their medications
    (Wilens et al. 2008). One survey reported that 23.3% of
    middle and high school students taking prescribed stimu-
    lants had been solicited to divert their medication to oth-
    ers at a rate that increased from middle school to high
    school (McCabe et al. 2004). A review of 161 elementary
    and high school students prescribed the stimulant MPH
    revealed that they had been asked to give or sell their
    medication to others (Musser et al. 1998). Data has
    shown that the diversion continues among college stu-
    dents. McCabe et al. found 54% of college students who
    were prescribed stimulants for ADHD had been
    approached to divert their medication (McCabe and Boyd
    2005). Nearly 29% of 334 college students had sold or
    given their medication to others (Upadhyaya et al. 2005).
    McCabe et al. (2005) examined the prevalence rates
    and correlates of nonprescription use of stimulants (Rita-
    lin, Adderall, or Dexedrine) among US college students
    and found evidence that misuse is more prevalent among
    particular subgroups of US college students and types of
    colleges. The lifetime prevalence of nonprescription
    stimulant use was 6.9%, past-year prevalence was 4.1%,
    and past-month prevalence was 2.1%. Multivariate
    analysis indicated that nonprescription use was higher
    among college students who were male, white, members
    of fraternities and sororities and earned lower grade point
    averages. Wilens et al. (2008) reported similar ´¼ündings.
    Rates were higher at colleges located in the northeastern
    region of the United States and colleges with more
    competitive admission standards. Nonprescription
    stimulant users were more likely to report use of alcohol,
    cigarettes, marijuana, ecstasy, cocaine, and other risky
    behaviors. Among college students, available evidence
    suggests that individuals who misuse MPH were more
    likely to be white, male, af´¼üliated with a formally
    organized fraternity, and more likely to use other illicit
    and illegal substances (Bogle and Smith 2009).
    A descriptive, nonexperimental, cross-sectional study
    examined the nonprescription use of stimulants among
    student pharmacists (Lord et al. 2003). Lifetime preva-
    lence of stimulant misuse was 7% and was more likely in
    students who were white, older, and fraternity or sorority
    members, whereas past-year misuse was more likely in
    whites and low academic achievers. A recent survey found
    that the misuse of prescription stimulants is also rampant
    among dental and dental hygiene students (McNiel et al.
    2011). The survey, which was mailed to dental education
    institutions in the south-central region of the United
    States, found that 12.4% of these students used a stimu-
    lant without a prescription and, of those, 70% took it to
    improve attention and/or concentration. The most com-
    monly reported stimulant medication used was Adderall
    (77%). The majority (87%) of the students obtained the
    medication through friends, and 90% began using the
    drug in college. Interestingly, 17% of the students sur-
    veyed felt it was easy to obtain stimulant medication for
    use at their school, and 17% thought it was a problem
    within their institution. The use, misuse, and diversion of
    prescription stimulants among middle and high school
    students were also examined by McCabe et al. (2005). In
    this study, the odds for nonprescription stimulant use
    were lower among African American students and higher
    among those students with no plans for attending college.
    These students also had the highest rates of alcohol and
    other drug use.
    The prevalence of prescription stimulant misuse in
    medical students is also high. In fact, discussion based
    websites such as Facebook, Medical School Forum, and
    The Student Doctor Network are rife with Adderall
    ÔÇ£expertsÔÇØ and informal question-and-answer sessions on
    the drug. An anonymous survey was administered to
    388 medical students (84.0% return rate) across all
    4 years of education at a public medical college. More
    664 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessnerthan 10% of medical students reported using stimulants
    to improve academic performance. ADHD was diag-
    nosed in 5.5% of students and 72.2% of those students
    were diagnosed after the age of 18 years (Tuttle et al.
    2010). This study suggests that medical students appear
    to be a relatively high-risk population for prescription
    stimulant misuse. Several of´¼ücials now say the problem
    is increasing in medical schools (Harris 2009). ÔÇ£During
    the last few years, the number of requests for ADD
    evaluations has hugely increased,ÔÇØ Paula Stoessel, Ph.D.,
    director of mental health services for physicians in
    training at the University of California, Los Angeles,
    David Geffen School of Medicine. ÔÇ£We make them
    [medical students] go through a lot before we hand out
    medication, but IÔÇÖve heard them talk about [obtaining
    Adderall prescriptions] in passing.ÔÇØ Clearly, the results
    emphasize the need for education about stimulants and
    their adverse side effects.
    Why are prescription stimulants
    misused?
    The reasons why prescription stimulants are misused are
    numerous and include achieving euphoria, and helping
    cope with stressful factors related to their educational
    environment. According to a survey of 334 ADHD-
    diagnosed college students taking prescription stimulants,
    25% misused their own prescription medications to get
    ÔÇ£highÔÇØ (Upadhyaya et al. 2005). Like cocaine, MPH
    inhibits the DAT, which increases synaptic levels of DA,
    and this is presumed to mediate MPHÔÇÖs reinforcing effects
    and abuse potential. In laboratory studies, it has been
    shown that animals will repeatedly administer MPH as
    they do cocaine (Kollins 2003), and humans receiving
    both drugs indicate a similar ÔÇ£highÔÇØ (Volkow et al. 1995).
    A frequent concern regarding the use of stimulants for
    ADHD is their mechanism of action, which increases DA
    and thus may increase the risk for overt, illicit drug use.
    However, research points to the conclusion that people of
    any age receiving a stimulant for ADHD have no greater
    risk for illicit substance abuse compared with the general
    population (Wilens 2003).
    Stimulants are especially popular at the end of a school
    term when students will often use the drugs to stay awake
    through the night to study for exams or complete aca-
    demic projects. In fact, prescription stimulants are most
    commonly misused to enhance school performance.
    According to a Web survey of 115 ADHD-diagnosed col-
    lege students, enhancing the ability to study outside of
    class was the primary motive for misuse (Rabiner et al.
    2009). Pressures such as a persistent desire to succeed
    academically, poor sleep habits due to large workloads,
    and the persistence of underlying social and ´¼ünancial
    demands may place students at an increased risk for mis-
    use of various drugs, including stimulants (Kadison 2005;
    Teter et al. 2005). Students who misused ADHD medica-
    tions generally felt that doing so was helpful. Thus, pre-
    scription stimulants developed to help children with
    ADHD improve their focus and attention are often mis-
    used by the patient, especially ADHD patients with con-
    duct disorder or comorbid substance abuse (Kollins
    2008). Moreover, students without ADHD misuse stimu-
    lants to improve performance or to induce euphoria. A
    web-based survey administered to medical and health
    profession students found that the most common reason
    for nonprescription stimulant use was to focus and con-
    centrate during studying (93.5%) (Herman et al. 2011).
    In this study, approximately 10.4% of students surveyed
    (45.2% female; 83.9% male; 83.9% Caucasian) have either
    used a stimulant or are currently using prescription stim-
    ulants, and the most commonly abused stimulant
    (71.4%) was d-AMP. A recent survey found that 70% of
    dental and dental hygiene students used a prescription
    stimulant nonmedically to improve attention and/or con-
    centration (McNiel et al. 2011). Student pharmacists
    (Lord et al. 2003) and medical students (Tuttle et al.
    2010) are also using stimulants to improve concentration
    and academic performance.
    Effects of prescription stimulants on
    cognition in ADHD
    Neuropsychological studies of ADHD children and adults
    indicate impairments in many cognitive areas including
    selective attention, memory, reaction time, information
    processing speed, and executive control function such as
    set-shifting, and working memory. The bene´¼üts of pre-
    scription stimulants for enhancing classroom manageabil-
    ity and increasing attention and academic productivity in
    children are well established. Prescription stimulants may
    increase the quality of note taking, scores on quizzes and
    worksheets, writing output, and homework completion.
    Nevertheless, they do not normalize the ability to learn
    and apply knowledge (Advokat 2010). In fact, it has been
    recognized over 30 years that there is little evidence that
    prescription stimulants such as MPH and AMP improve
    the academic achievement of ADHD-diagnosed children.
    Children with ADHD have a consistently lower full-scale
    IQ than normal controls. They score signi´¼ücantly lower
    on reading and arithmetic tests, use more remedial aca-
    demic services, and are more likely to be placed in a spe-
    cial education class, or repeat a grade compared with
    controls. They also take more years to complete high
    school and have lower rates of college attendance
    (Advokat 2010). Thus, prescription stimulants have only
    a modest impact on these outcomes.
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 665
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDThe ´¼ürst review to describe the general academic func-
    tioning of adults with ADHD summarized the results
    from 23 studies (Weyandt and DuPaul 2006). ADHD-
    diagnosed college students were found to have signi´¼ü-
    cantly lower grade point averages, report more ÔÇ£academic
    problemsÔÇØ and to be less likely to graduate from college.
    Nevertheless, ADHD-diagnosed college students did not
    differ in IQ from those without ADHD, and were shown
    to be able to meet the demands of college courses. On
    psychological tests, they showed signi´¼ücant de´¼ücits in
    attention, but were not different from normal students on
    other measures, such as the ability to be ´¼éexible and to
    maintain performance, as task demands varied (Weyandt
    and DuPaul 2006). More recent reports have reached sim-
    ilar conclusions. Interestingly, like elementary and high
    school students, college students with ADHD are less
    likely to reach the same academic level as their non-
    ADHD counterparts, even when they use stimulant medi-
    cations. Thus, stimulant medications do not necessarily
    equalize academic achievement in the typical adult with
    ADHD.
    A recent controlled, cross-sectional study evaluated the
    effects of stimulants on cognition in adults with ADHD
    and found that treated ADHD subjects had signi´¼ücantly
    better scores on measures of IQ than did untreated
    patients (Biederman et al. 2012). Thus, either good cogni-
    tive functioning may be a determinant of seeking treat-
    ment or stimulant treatment may improve cognition in
    adults with ADHD. When ADHD studies address the
    issue of cognition, they usually demonstrate that treated
    patients perform better than untreated patients on neuro-
    psychological tests or measures after they are treated.
    Whether treatment normalizes neurocognitive perfor-
    mance is rarely addressed. In fact, adults with ADHD are
    less likely to attain the same educational levels as those
    without the diagnosis relative to what would be predicted
    based on their IQ, and this outcome does not appear to
    be improved by stimulant medication. In one recent
    study, for example, although 84% of ADHD-diagnosed
    adults were statistically expected to be college graduates,
    only 50% reached this level of education (Biederman
    et al. 2008a,b). Gualtieri and Johnson (2008) conducted a
    cross-sectional study of ADHD patients treated with
    different ADHD drugs (Adderall XR, atomoxetine,
    Concerta) (Adderall XR is an extended-release formu-
    lation with duration of action of approximately 10ÔÇô12 h.
    This is signi´¼ücantly longer than the duration of action of
    most methylphenidate formulations, with the exception of
    Concerta. Immediate-release methylphenidate lasts at
    most for 6 h). PatientsÔÇÖ performance on a computerized
    neurocognitive screening battery was compared with
    untreated ADHD patients and normal controls. Signi´¼ü-
    cant differences were detected between normal and
    untreated ADHD patients. Treated patients performed
    better than untreated patients but remained signi´¼ücantly
    impaired compared with normal subjects. Thus, even after
    optimal treatment, neurocognitive impairments persisted
    in the ADHD patients.
    It has never been established that the cognitive effects
    of stimulant drugs are central to their therapeutic utility.
    In fact, although ADHD medications are effective for the
    behavioral components of the disorder, little information
    exists concerning their effects on cognition. Barkley and
    Cunningham (1978) summarized 17 short-term research
    studies ranging from 2 weeks to 6 months, and found
    stimulant medications produced little improvement in the
    academic performance of hyperkinetic ADHD children.
    The drugs appeared to reduce disruptive behavior rather
    than improve academic performance. Stimulant drugs do
    improve the ability (even without ADHD) to focus and
    pay attention. One function, which is reliably improved
    by stimulant medications, is sustained attention, or vigi-
    lance. Stimulants improve sustained, focused attention,
    but ÔÇ£selective attentionÔÇØ and ÔÇ£distractibilityÔÇØ may be
    worsened, possibly because of a drug induced increase in
    impulsivity. Both AMP and MPH do not improve (and
    may even impair) short-term acquisition of information.
    In addition, AMP and MPH do not improve, and may
    impair ÔÇ£cognitive ´¼éexibilityÔÇØ as assessed with tests such as
    the Wisconsin Card Sort and Attentional Set-Shifting
    tasks. MPH has been shown to improve performance on
    an auditory arithmetic task, the Paced Auditory Serial
    Addition Task, in adults with ADHD relative to control
    subjects (Schweitzer et al. 2004). AMP and MPH might
    improve long-term retention of information, if the drugs
    are active during a period in which memory is being
    ÔÇ£consolidated.ÔÇØ However, this may only occur in situa-
    tions where retention is already suboptimal.
    Effects of stimulants on cognition in
    individuals without ADHD
    Recognition that ADHD persists into adulthood has sub-
    stantially increased the prescription stimulant treatment
    of adults with the disorder (see above). It has also
    resulted in a corresponding escalation of nonprescription
    stimulant use in many college students con´¼ürmed by
    numerous surveys. Studies consistently show that students
    report using stimulant medications, legally or illicitly, to
    improve academic performance, speci´¼ücally to increase
    concentration and the ability to stay up longer and study.
    Intuitively, it would seem logical that drugs that improve
    attention and concentration should also promote learning
    and academic achievement. Inherent in terms like ÔÇ£cogni-
    tive enhancers,ÔÇØ ÔÇ£smart drugs,ÔÇØ and ÔÇ£neuroenhancersÔÇØ is
    the assumption that MPH and d-AMP enhance cognition.
    666 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. KirchgessnerMajor magazines such as The New Yorker have reported a
    trend toward growing use of prescription stimulants by
    college students for ÔÇ£neuroenhancementÔÇØ. In fact, some
    students are faking ADHD to gain access to prescription
    stimulant medication, which has led to a shortage of
    ADHD drugs such as Adderall (Mitchell 2012). Unfortu-
    nately, media reports appear to condone this behavior as
    95% of articles mentioned at least one possible bene´¼üt of
    using prescription drugs for neuroenhancement, but only
    58% mentioned any risks or side effects (Partridge et al.
    2011). Duke University recently enacted a new policy pro-
    hibiting the nonmedical use of prescription stimulants for
    any academic purposes (McLaughlin 2012). Students
    received an email stating policy changes including, ÔÇ£The
    unauthorized use of prescription medication to enhance
    academic performance has been added to the de´¼ünition
    of Cheating.ÔÇØ In the past, the use of such drugs without a
    prescription was only a violation under the UniversityÔÇÖs
    drug policy. Oddly, the assumption that prescription
    stimulants are truly ÔÇ£cognitive enhancersÔÇØ is not really
    questioned. Stimulants reduce hyperactivity, impulsivity,
    and inattention in children and adults with ADHD, so it
    has been assumed that these drugs enhance long-term
    intellectual performance. However, contrary to simple
    implicit assumptions found in bioethics and media dis-
    courses, there are actually only a few studies on the
    enhancement effects of ÔÇ£cognitive enhancersÔÇØ in individu-
    als without ADHD.
    Smith and Farah (2011) reviewed data on prescription
    stimulants as neuroenhancers from over forty laboratory
    studies involving healthy, nonelderly adults. Most of the
    studies looked at one of three types of cognition: learn-
    ing, working memory, and cognitive control. Effects of
    d-AMP or MPH on cognition were assessed by a variety
    of tasks (Table 1). A typical learning task asks subjects
    to memorize a list of paired words; an hour, a few days,
    or a week later, subjects are presented with the ´¼ürst
    words in the pairs and asked to come up with the sec-
    ond. In general, with single exposures of verbal material,
    the studies on learning showed that no bene´¼üts are seen
    immediately following learning, but later recall and rec-
    ognition are enhanced. Of the six articles reporting on
    memory performance (Rapoport et al. 1978; Soetens
    et al. 1993; Camp-Bruno and Herting 1994; Fleming
    et al. 1995; Unrug et al. 1997; Zeeuws and Soetens
    2007), encompassing eight separate experiments, only
    one of the experiments yielded signi´¼ücant memory
    enhancement on short delays (Rapoport et al. 1978). In
    contrast, retention was reliably enhanced by d-AMP
    when subjects were tested after longer delays, with recall
    improved after 1 h through 1 week (Soetens et al. 1993,
    1995; Zeeuws and Soetens 2007). These data suggest that
    when people are given rote-learning tasks their perfor-
    mance is improved by stimulants. The bene´¼üts were
    more apparent in studies where subjects had been asked
    to remember information for several days or longer.
    However, studies only found a correlation with rote
    memory tasks, not complex memory, which is more
    likely to appear on college exams.
    In contrast to the types of memory, which are long
    lasting and formed as a result of learning, working mem-
    ory is a temporary store of information that plays a role
    in executive function. Several studies have assessed the
    effect of MPH or d-AMP on tasks examining various
    aspects of working memory (Sahakian and Owen 1992;
    Oken et al. 1995; Elliott et al. 1997; Mehta et al. 2000;
    Barch and Carter 2005; Silber et al. 2006; Clatworthy
    et al. 2009) (see Table 1). One classic approach to the
    assessment of working memory is the span task, in which
    a series of items is presented to the subject for repetition,
    transcription, or recognition. A spatial span task, in which
    the subjects must retain and reproduce the order in which
    boxes in a scattered spatial arrangement change color was
    employed by Elliott et al. (1997) to assess the effects of
    MPH on working memory. For the subjects in the group
    who received placebo ´¼ürst, MPH increased spatial span.
    However, for the subjects who received MPH ´¼ürst, there
    was a nonsigni´¼ücant opposite trend. The authors noted
    that the subjects in the ´¼ürst group performed at an overall
    lower level, and so, this may have contributed to the lar-
    ger enhancement effect for less able subjects. Barch and
    Carter (2005) obtained similar results and Mehta et al.
    (2000) found evidence of greater accuracy with MPH. In
    the study by Mehta et al. (2000), the effect depended on
    subjectsÔÇÖ working memory ability: the lower a subjectÔÇÖs
    score on placebo, the greater the improvement on MPH.
    In contrast to the three previous studies, Bray et al.
    (2004) reported that MPH does not improve the cogni-
    tive function of sleep-deprived young adults. In sum, the
    evidence concerning stimulant effects of working memory
    is mixed, with some ´¼ündings of enhancement and some
    null results, although no ´¼ündings of overall performance
    impairment (Smith and Farah 2011). However, the small
    effects were mainly evident in subjects who had low
    cognitive performance to start with, showing that the
    drug is more effective at correcting de´¼ücits than
    ÔÇ£enhancing performance.ÔÇØ Farah et al. (2009) recently
    examined the effect of Adderall upon creativity, a
    component of cognition stimulants are suspected of sti-
    ´¼éing, in a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. They
    found that the drug enhanced creativity on speci´¼üc tasks,
    but the amount of enhancement depended upon the
    baseline performance of individuals: lower-performing
    individuals were more enhanced than high-performers.
    Thus, the drugs do not offer as much help to people with
    greater intellectual abilities.
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 667
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDTable 1. Overview of effects of prescription stimulants on cognitive performance in adults without ADHD.
    Study Tests Finding
    Barch and Carter
    (2005)
    Spatial working memory Decrease in reaction time
    Stroop test Decrease in response time
    Breitenstein et al.
    (2004)
    Probabilistic learning Steeper increase in hits and decrease in misses across learning sessions;
    increase in retention after more than 1 year
    Breitenstein et al.
    (2006)
    Probabilistic learning Steeper learning curve
    Brignell et al.
    (2007)
    Single-exposure verbal learning At 1 week improved recognition
    Brumaghim and
    Klormart (1998)
    Associative learning: word pairs No effect
    Burns et al. (1967) Associative learning: location of stimulus and
    response
    Slower rate of learning
    Callaway (1983) Item recognition No effect
    Camp-Bruno and
    Herting (1994)
    Repeated-exposure verbal learning 1 h: no effect; 2 h: borderline effect
    Camp-Bruno and
    Herting (1994)
    Single-exposure verbal learning Up to 2.5 h: no effect
    Clatworthy et al.
    (2009)
    Spatial working memory No effect
    Reversal learning No effect
    Cooper et al.
    (2005)
    Continuous performance test (double version) 5 min: decrease in reaction time; decrease in errors of omission
    de Wit et al.
    (2000)
    Stop-signal task No effect
    de Wit et al.
    (2002)
    Repeated-exposure verbal learning 25 min: no effect
    Digit span Increase in performance
    Go/no-go Decrease in number of false alarms
    Delay of grati´¼ücation No effect
    Dodds et al.
    (2008)
    Reversal learning No effect
    Elliott et al. (1997) Spatial span Decrease in errors
    Spatial working memory Decrease in errors
    Attentional set-shifting No effect
    Verbal ´¼éuency No effect
    Sequence generation No effect
    New Tower of London No effect
    Tower of London Relative decrease in accuracy
    Fillmore et al.
    (2005)
    Stop-signal task No effect
    n-back Increase in processing rate
    Fitzpatrick et al.
    (1988)
    Item recognition: stimulus evaluation/
    response selection task
    Decrease in reaction time
    Fleming et al.
    (1995)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 20 min: no effect on single-exposure verbal learning
    Continuous performance test 5 min: decrease in reaction time
    Spatial working memory No effect
    Wisconsin Card Sorting Test No effect
    Verbal ´¼éuency No effect
    Hurst et al. (1969) Associative learning: word pairs Increase in retention after 1 week delay
    Kennedy et al.
    (1990)
    Item recognition No effect
    Grammatical reasoning No effect
    Klorman et al.
    (1984)
    Continuous performance test (BX version) 45 min: decrease in reaction time; 12.5/45 min: decrease in errors of
    omission
    Koelega (1993) Vigilance performance Improves the overall level of vigilance performance and prevents the
    decrement that occurs over time under normal circumstances
    Kumari et al.
    (1997)
    Motor sequence learning No effect
    Motor sequence learning No effect
    (Continued)
    668 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. KirchgessnerThe third type of cognition is cognitive control. Cogni-
    tive control is a broad concept that refers to guidance of
    cognitive processes in situations where the most natural,
    automatic, or available action is not necessarily the cor-
    rect one (Smith and Farah 2011). Attention and working
    memory are thought to rely on cognitive control and loss
    of cognitive control is a major component of many neu-
    ropsychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia. The effects
    of MPH and d-AMP have been determined on several
    tests used to study cognitive control, including the go/no-
    go task, the stop-signal task, and the Flanker test. In gen-
    eral, the effects of stimulants on cognitive control are not
    robust, but MPH and d-AMP appear to enhance cogni-
    tive control in some tasks for some people, especially
    those less likely to perform well on cognitive control tasks
    (Smith and Farah 2011). The results of these studies cur-
    rently provide limited support for the enthusiastic por-
    trayals of cognitive enhancement.
    The neural basis of error processing has become a key
    research interest in cognitive neuroscience. Recently, a
    single dose of MPH was shown to improve the ability of
    healthy volunteers to consciously detect performance
    errors (Hester et al. 2012). Furthermore, this behavioral
    effect was associated with a strengthening of activation
    Table 1. Continued.
    Study Tests Finding
    Makris et al.
    (2007)
    Item recognition Proportion correct sustained across multiple trials
    Mattay et al.
    (1996)
    Wisconsin Card Sorting Test No effect
    Mattay et al.
    (2000)
    n-back No effect
    Mattay et al.
    (2003)
    n-back No effect
    Wisconsin Card Sorting Test No effect
    Mehta et al.
    (2000)
    Spatial working memory Decrease in between-search errors
    Mintzer and
    Grif´¼üths (2007)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 2 h: improved recognition; no effect on recall
    n-back No effect
    Item recognition No effect
    Oken et al. (1995) Digit span No effect
    Rapoport et al.
    (1978)
    Single-exposure verbal learning; continuous
    performance test (BX version)
    10 min: improved recall; decrease in errors of omission
    Rogers et al.
    (1999)
    Attentional set-shifting Increase in intradimensional shift errors; decrease in extradimensional shift
    errors; increase in response latencies
    Schmedje et al.
    (1988)
    Digit span No effect
    Pattern memory No effect
    Schroeder et al.
    (1987)
    Strategic choice task Decrease in changeover rate
    Servan-Schreiber
    et al. (1998)
    Flanker task Decrease in response time; increase in accuracy
    Silber et al. (2006) Digit span No effect
    Trail Making Test No effect
    Soetens et al.
    (1993)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 20 min: no effect; 1 hÔÇô3 days: improved long-term retention
    Soetens et al.
    (1995)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 1 hÔÇô1 week: improved long-term retention in free recall; 1 week: improved
    recognition
    Strauss et al.
    (1984)
    Associative learning: word pairs; continuous
    performance test (double version)
    No effect; 45 min: decrease in reaction time; 45 min: decrease in errors of
    omission
    Unrug et al.
    (1997)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 20 min: no effect
    Ward et al. (1997) Motor sequence learning; item recognition No effect; decrease in reaction time
    Weitzner (1965) Associative learning: word pairs Improved performance only when pairs were uniquely semantically related
    Willett (1962) Repeated-exposure verbal learning Decrease in number of trials to reach criterion
    Zeeuws and
    Soetens (2007)
    Single-exposure verbal learning 30 min: no effect; 1 hÔÇô1 day: improved long-term retention
    Table adapted with permission from Smith and Farah (2011), Copyright 2011 by the American Psychological Association. The use of APA informa-
    tion does not imply endorsement by APA.
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 669

    differences in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and
    inferior parietal lobe during the MPH condition for
    errors made with versus without awareness. How the
    brain monitors ongoing behavior for performance errors
    is a central question of cognitive neuroscience. Dimin-
    ished awareness of performance errors limits the extent to
    which humans engage in corrective behavior and has been
    linked to loss of insight in ADHD and drug addiction.
    As it remains unclear whether stimulant medication
    has the same effect on healthy individuals as for those
    with ADHD, it is possible that many reported effects of
    prescription stimulants in healthy individuals may stem
    from placebo effects. Looby and Earleywine (2011) exam-
    ined whether placebo effects in´¼éuence reports of subjec-
    tive mood and cognitive performance among college
    students who endorsed several risk factors for prescription
    stimulant misuse (e.g., low grade point average, frater-
    nity/sorority involvement, binge drinking). Interestingly,
    participants believed that they had better ability to focus
    and persevere, particularly for a sustained amount of
    time, when they expected to receive MPH (Looby and
    Earleywine 2011). This is similar to circumstances in
    which participants may engage in nonmedical-stimulant
    use to study or cram for extended hours. On the other
    hand, when experimental participants did not expect to
    receive MPH, their attention appeared disrupted resulting
    in inconsistent reaction times throughout the CPT. Inter-
    estingly, subjective feelings of being high and stimulated
    were produced solely by expecting to receive MPH. This
    ´¼ünding is important to consider when examining initia-
    tion and maintenance of nonmedical prescription stimu-
    lant use. As motives for nonprescription stimulant use
    include the desire to feel high (Barrett et al. 2005), it is
    likely that individuals who use a stimulant for this pur-
    pose will consequently feel high due to these demon-
    strated placebo effects, which will likely maintain misuse
    of the drug.
    Prescription stimulant misuse in
    athletes
    ADHD is a controversial problem in sport as participants
    with this disorder often require banned stimulants while
    competing. Many of the governing bodies of competitive
    sports have developed regulations that limit the use of
    stimulant medications to treat ADHD. In other cases,
    stimulant use is allowed in the setting of a documented
    diagnosis of ADHD. Most sports organizations around
    the world now follow the guidelines set forth by the
    World Anti-Doping Agency (WADA). According to this
    document, the diagnosis of ADHD is to be made by
    ÔÇ£experienced cliniciansÔÇØ and in accordance to the DSM-
    IV. Stimulant medications are considered to be a
    ÔÇ£medical best practice treatmentÔÇØ that do require the
    athlete to ´¼üle a therapeutic use exemption (TUE). A TUE
    gives athletes with medical diagnoses an exemption to
    use a drug normally prohibited by MLB, to treat a
    legitimately diagnosed medical condition. WADA
    recommends reassessments of continued treatment every
    3ÔÇô4 months. Other organizations, such as the National
    College Athletic Association (NCAA) and individual
    professional leagues, such as the National Football League
    (NFL) and Major League Baseball (MLB), have developed
    their own regulations.
    The NCAA does not require that physicians prescribe a
    trial of nonstimulant medications before prescribing stim-
    ulants, only that the prescribing physician considers non-
    stimulants ´¼ürst. The NCAA acknowledges that
    nonstimulant medication may not be as effective as stim-
    ulant medications in treating ADHD. In contrast to the
    NCAA regulations, athletes who are also participating in
    events governed by the International Olympic Committee
    (IOC) and/or WADA are not allowed to use stimulant
    medications, even with a TUE. These organizations
    require that the athlete with ADHD on stimulant medica-
    tions stop taking these medication or risk disquali´¼ücation
    (Putukian et al. 2011).
    It has been reported that MLB players are using an
    ADHD diagnosis to evade the AMP ban (Associated Press
    2009). According to records MLB of´¼ücials turned over to
    congressional investigators as part of George MitchellÔÇÖs
    probe into steroid use in baseball, the number of players
    getting ÔÇ£therapeutic use exemptionsÔÇØ from baseballÔÇÖs AMP
    ban jumped in 1 year from 28 to 103 ÔÇô which means that,
    suddenly, 7.6% of the 1354 players on major-league rosters
    have been diagnosed with ADHD. MLB banned AMP in
    2006. The prevalence of ADHD in athletes has not been
    studied, although there is no reason to believe it would dif-
    fer from the general population. Thus, 2ÔÇô3 times the usual
    adult rate of ADHD in baseball players is alarming. Ath-
    letes may see stimulants as a way to help maintain physical
    ´¼ütness for their competitive sport or to improve their con-
    centration. Certainly some of the players getting prescrip-
    tions for ADHD medications may have a legitimate
    medical need and without treatment, players manifesting
    the symptoms of untreated ADHD would be at a disadvan-
    tage to non-ADHD players. A therapeutic dose of MPH
    will bene´¼üt concentration, and may improve motor coor-
    dination. Prescription stimulants to treat ADHD could be
    used as performance enhancing drugs (PEDs); however, a
    proper diagnosis would prevent athletes from abusing the
    TUE status to ÔÇ£cheat within the rules.ÔÇØ
    Some athletes will only take medications episodically for
    school testing or for studying purposes. Others may feel
    that their sport performance is improved on stimulants,
    whereas others may temporarily stop taking them so
    670 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessnerthat their sports play is more random and unfocused,
    which they feel improves their performance (Pelham et al.
    1990).
    Potential adverse affects of chronic
    stimulant use
    ADHD is now recognized as a chronic disorder that con-
    tinues into adulthood; therefore, some individuals take
    stimulants such as MPH and d-AMP for years. The medi-
    cal literature provides abundant data to support the
    potentially positive effect of stimulants for the majority of
    children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD, and stimu-
    lants have been considered to be relatively safe (Elia et al.
    1999; Brown et al. 2005). However, reports of adverse
    events in conjunction with the use of these drugs have
    raised concern about their safety.
    Large doses of stimulants can lead to psychosis, sei-
    zures, and cardiovascular events. The induction of schizo-
    phrenic-like states in AMP abusers is well documented,
    although the onset of such states in children on pre-
    scribed doses of stimulant medication is observed far less
    often (Polchert and Morse 1985; Masand et al. 1991;
    Murray 1998). Surles et al. (2002) published a case report
    of psychotic reactions to AMP (10 mg/day) in an adoles-
    cent ADHD patient. The patient displayed many of the
    characteristics of AMP-induced psychosis including visual
    hallucinations, delusions, anorexia, ´¼éattening of affect,
    and insomnia. It is thought that the mechanism of AMP-
    induced psychosis is mediated by dopaminergic excess. As
    the patientÔÇÖs symptoms disappeared when taken off the
    stimulant medication, it suggests that the psychosis was
    indeed secondary to AMP.
    The most commonly observed cardiovascular effects
    linked with ADHD stimulant medications include hyper-
    tension and tachycardia. In addition, cardiomyopathy,
    cardiac dysrhythmias, and necrotizing vasculitis have been
    described. In February 2005, the brand medication Adder-
    all XR (Shire BioChem Inc, Quebec, Canada) was with-
    drawn from the Canadian market by Health Canada. Case
    reports on serious cardiovascular adverse drug reactions
    (ADRs), sudden death, and psychiatric disorders led regu-
    latory agencies to warn against the use of MPH in the
    pediatric population in 2006 and 2007 (European Medi-
    cines Agency 2007). In 2006, warnings were also linked to
    atomoxetine use due to reports of hepatotoxicity and
    suicidal thoughts in children. These concerns received
    glaring attention in 2006 and led the US Food and Drug
    Administration advisory committee to propose placing a
    black box warning concerning sudden death on
    psychostimulants in response to ADR reports.
    Adderall use is associated with myocardial infarction
    and even sudden death (Gandhi et al. 2005; Jiao et al.
    2009). Gandhi et al. (2005) reported the case of a
    15-year-old male subject who suffered a myocardial
    infarction after taking two 20 mg tablets of Adderall. Jiao
    et al. (2009) reported a second case of a 20-year-old
    ADHD college freshman with myocardial infarction after
    taking two 15-mg tablets of Adderall XR. Recently,
    Sylvester and Agarwala (2012) reported another case of a
    15-year-old male subject who suffered a myocardial
    infarction after starting Adderall XR. The patient was
    otherwise in good health with no previous cardiac abnor-
    malities and improved with cessation of medication. The
    ´¼ündings of the case have been disputed (Rosenthal 2012).
    In addition, a recent report by Alsidawi et al. (2011)
    discusses the case of a 19-year-old female subject with
    Adderall overdose induced inverted-Takotsubo cardiomy-
    opathy, also known as stress-induced cardiomyopathy.
    The patient was brought to the emergency department
    after ingesting 30 Adderall tablets. She complained of
    pressure like chest pain and shortness of breath. Her car-
    diac enzymes were elevated, but the electrocardiogram
    was unremarkable. Echocardiography identi´¼üed a low
    ejection fraction of 25ÔÇô35% with severe hyperkinetic apex
    and akinetic base consistent with the diagnosis of
    inverted-TTC. Her symptoms resolved in 24 h. Drug-
    induced-Takotsubo cardiomyopathy has been previously
    reported and is mainly attributed to sympathetic oversti-
    mulation (Amariles 2011). In this case, the patient over-
    dosed on Adderall, which is a sympathomimetic drug.
    The mechanisms for AMP-induced cardiac injury are pos-
    tulated to be similar to those seen with cocaine, which
    include coronary spasm, prothrombotic state, accelerated
    atherosclerosis due to endothelial injury, and direct myo-
    cardial (Chen 2007). Inappropriate dosing or taking with
    alcohol increases the risk of serious cardiovascular side
    effects like myocardial infarction, even without underlying
    cardiovascular risk factors.
    Unfortunately, there are few long-term studies (i.e.,
    longer than 24 months) on the use of stimulants for the
    management of ADHD; therefore, the precise long-term
    effects ÔÇô either adverse or positive ÔÇô remain unknown.
    A recent study (Vitiello et al. 2011) suggests that the
    chronic use of stimulant medication to treat ADHD in
    children does not appear to increase the risk for high
    blood pressure in the long term, but it may have modest
    effects on heart rate. The MTA study found that stimulant
    medication does not appear to increase the risk for
    abnormal elevations in blood pressure or heart rate over a
    10-year period; however, the effect of stimulants on heart
    rate can be detected even after years of use (Vitiello et al.
    2011). The effect on heart rate may be clinically signi´¼ücant
    for individuals who have underlying heart conditions.
    A cohort study sought to determine whether use of
    MPH in adults is associated with elevated rates of serious
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 671
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDcardiovascular events compared with rates in nonusers
    (Schelleman et al. 2012). All new MPH users with at least
    180 days of prior enrollment were identi´¼üed. Initiation of
    MPH was associated with a 1.8-fold increase in risk of sud-
    den death or ventricular arrhythmia; however, the lack of a
    dose response relationship suggested that this association
    might not be a causal one. A recent study by Habel and
    colleagues (Habel et al. 2011), which compared approxi-
    mately 150,000 adults prescribed ADHD medication with
    approximately 300,000 nonusers, found no evidence of a
    link between ADHD medication and cardiovascular risk
    (myocardial infarction, sudden death, or stroke). Although
    the student enrolled adults, the same group also has
    reported a similar lack of signi´¼ücant association between
    serious cardiovascular events and use of ADHD medica-
    tions in children and younger adults (Cooper et al. 2011).
    These ´¼ündings support the ´¼ünal decision of the US Food
    and Drug Administration committee to not to place a
    black box warning for all children and adults, but to
    pursue further research. However, the study by Habel et al.
    (2011) has limitations stemming from its focus on the
    most severe cardiovascular event. The databases were not
    used to examine other cardiovascular adverse effects, such
    as palpitations and dyspnea, which, although less severe,
    are nonetheless alarming to patients.
    Additional potential ADRs associated with stimulant use
    are important to note including abdominal pain, anorexia,
    constipation, dizziness, dry mouth, headache, insomnia,
    jitteriness, irritability, nausea, and palpitations (Greydanus
    and Strasburger 2006). College students with ADHD who
    misuse prescribed stimulants also reported hyperactivity
    symptoms as a common adverse event. Of particular
    signi´¼ücance to athletes, many stimulants utilized in treat-
    ing ADHD may increase core temperature (Piper et al.
    2005), possibly increasing risk of heart injury. These agents
    may also mask signs and symptoms of fatigue and allow
    for a longer duration of exercise with elevated temperature
    in excess of 40??C. Thus, in situations of increased exoge-
    nous heat stress, stimulants should be used with caution.
    Conclusion
    Although prescription stimulants have been shown to be
    relatively safe and effective in managing the symptoms of
    ADHD, there exists a signi´¼ücant potential for misuse. The
    data are clear that individuals with and without ADHD,
    including athletes misuse stimulants to enhance perfor-
    mance. Although stimulants may improve an individualÔÇÖs
    performance when given a rote-learning task, they do not
    offer as much help to people with greater intellectual abil-
    ities. Stimulants do not increase IQ (Advokat et al. 2008).
    In fact, very little is known about the effects of nonpre-
    scription stimulants on cognitive enhancement outside of
    the student population, although it is frequently reported
    in newspaper articles. Thus, the rumored effects of ÔÇ£smart
    drugsÔÇØ may be a false promise, as research suggests that
    stimulants are more effective at correcting de´¼ücits than
    ÔÇ£enhancing performance.ÔÇØ Moreover, students are taking
    unnecessary risks including the potential for harmful side
    effects, which may cause sudden death. This requires edu-
    cation on the proper use of stimulants and on the signs
    and symptoms of misuse and the health risks associated
    with misuse. It is important that students with prescription
    stimulants understand that they are the main source of
    diversion to other students, and should receive education
    in the prevention of stimulant diversion. Health centers
    should aim to recognize students who are misusing stimu-
    lants because they may present with a variety of signs
    including insisting on a larger dose, and demanding more
    drug during times within the academic year, such as dur-
    ing ´¼ünals. Students with past or active drug abuse patterns
    should not be prescribed stimulants, as they are more
    likely to divert their prescription stimulants. It is also
    important that athletes be warned that the NCAA, the US
    Olympic Committee, and the IOC ban MPH. As a result,
    education on the proper use of stimulants and on the signs
    and symptoms of misuse is an imperative.
    Acknowledgments
    The development of this work was supported by the
    Global Neuroscience Initiative Foundation (GNIF).
    Con´¼éict of Interest
    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
    Authors contributions: All authors participated in the
    preparation of the manuscript, and read and approved
    the ´¼ünal manuscript.
    References
    Advokat, C. 2010. What are the cognitive effects of stimulant
    medications? Emphasis on adults with attention-de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
    34:1256ÔÇô1266.
    Advokat, C. D., M. S. Guidry, and B. A. Martino. 2008. Licit
    and illicit use of medications for attention de´¼ücit
    hyperactivity disorder in undergraduate college students.
    J. Am. Coll. Health 56:601ÔÇô607.
    Alsidawi, S., J. Muth, and J. Wilkin. 2011. Adderall induced
    inverted-Takotsubo cardiomyopathy. Catheter. Cardiovasc.
    Interv. 78:910ÔÇô913.
    Amariles, P. 2011. A comprehensive literature search: drugs as
    possible triggers of Takotsubo cardiomyopathy. Curr. Clin.
    Pharmacol. 6:1ÔÇô11.
    672 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. KirchgessnerAmerican Psychiatric Association. 2000. DSM-IV-TR.
    American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
    Associated Press. 2009.ADHD exemptions on rise in MLB.
    ESPN, New York, NY.
    Barch, D. M., and C. S. Carter. 2005. Amphetamine improves
    cognitive function in medicated individuals with
    schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Res.
    77:43ÔÇô58.
    Barkley, R. A., and C. E. Cunningham. 1978. Do stimulant
    drugs improve the academic performance of hyperkinetic
    children? A review of outcome studies. Clin. Pediatr.
    (Phila.) 17:85ÔÇô92.
    Barrett, S. P., C. Darredeau, L. K. Brody, and R. O. Pihl. 2005.
    Characteristics of methylphenidate misuse in a university
    student sample. Can. J. Psychiatry 50:457ÔÇô461.
    Biederman, J., M. C. Monuteaux, E. Mick, T. Spencer, T. E.
    Wilens, J. M. Silva, et al. 2006. Young adult outcome of
    attention de´¼ücit hyperactivity disorder: a controlled
    10 year prospective follow-up study. Psychol. Med.
    36:167ÔÇô179.
    Biederman, J., C. R. Petty, R. Fried, R. Kaiser, C. R. Dolan, S.
    Schoenfeld, et al. 2008a. Educational and occupational
    underattainment in adults with attention-de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder: a controlled study. J. Clin. Psychiatry
    69:1217ÔÇô1222.
    Biederman, J., L. J. Seidman, C. R. Petty, R. Fried, A. E. Doyle,
    D. R. Cohen, et al. 2008b. Effects of stimulant medication
    on neuropsychological functioning in young adults with
    attention de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry
    69:1150ÔÇô1156.
    Biederman, J., R. Fried, C. R. Petty, A. Henin, J. Wozniak, L.
    Corkum, et al. 2012. Examining the association between
    stimulant treatment and cognitive outcomes across the life
    cycle of adults with attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder:
    a controlled cross-sectional study. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis.
    200:69ÔÇô75.
    Bloom, B., and R. A. Cohen. 2007. Summary health statistics
    for U.S. children: National Health Interview Survey, 2006.
    Statistics VH 234:1ÔÇô79.
    Bogle, K. E., and B. H. Smith. 2009. Illcit methylphenidate
    use: a review of prevalence, availability, pharmacology,
    and consequences. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 2:157ÔÇô
    176.
    Bray, C. L., K. S. Cahill, J. T. Oshier, C. S. Peden, D. W.
    Theriaque, T. R. Flotte, et al. 2004. Methylphenidate does
    not improve cognitive function in healthy sleep-deprived
    young adults. J. Invest. Med. 52:192ÔÇô201.
    Breitenstein, C., S. Wailke, S. Bushuven, S. Kamping, P.
    Zwitserlood, E. B. Ringelstein, et al. 2004. D-amphetamine
    boosts language learning independent of its cardiovascular
    and motor arousing effects. Neuropsychopharmacology
    29:1704ÔÇô1714.
    Breitenstein, C., A. Floel, C. Korsukewitz, S. Waike, S.
    Bushuven, and S. Knecht. 2006. A shift paradigm: from
    noradrenergic to dopaminergic modulation of learning? J.
    Neurol. Sci. 248:42ÔÇô47.
    Brignell, C. M., J. Rosenthal, and H. V. Curran. 2007.
    Pharmacological manipulations of arousal and memory for
    emotional material: effects of a single dose of
    methylphenidate or lorazepam. J. Psychopharmacol.
    21:673ÔÇô683.
    Brown, R. T., R. W. Amler, W. S. Freeman, J. M. Perrin, M.
    T. Stein, H. M. Feldman, et al. 2005. Treatment of
    attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder: overview of the
    evidence. Pediatrics 115:e749ÔÇôe757.
    Brumaghim, J. T., and R. Klormart. 1998. MethylphenidateÔÇÖs
    effects on paired-association learning and event-related
    potentials of young adults. Psychophysiology 35:73ÔÇô85.
    Burns, J. T., R. F. House, F. C. Fensch, and J. G. Miller. 1967.
    Effects of magnesium pemoline and dextroamphetamine on
    human learning. Science 155:849ÔÇô851.
    Callaway, E. 1983. Presidential address. 1982: the
    pharmacology of human information processing.
    Psychophysiology 20:359ÔÇô370.
    Camp-Bruno, J. A., and R. L. Herting. 1994. Cognitive effects
    of milacemide and methylphenidate in healthy young adults.
    Psychopharmacology 115:46ÔÇô52.
    Center for Disease Control and Prevention. 2005a. Mental
    health in the United States. Prevalence of diagnosis and
    medication treatment for attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity
    disorder ÔÇô United States, 2003. MMWR Morb. Mortal.
    Wkly. Rep. 54:842ÔÇô847.
    Center for Disease Control and Prevention. 2005b. Prevalence
    of diagnosis and medication treatment for attention de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder ÔÇô United States, 2003. MMWR
    Morbid. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 54:842ÔÇô847.
    Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2010. Increasing
    prevalence of parent-reported attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity
    disorder among children ÔÇô United States, 2003 and 2007.
    MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 59:1439ÔÇô1443.
    Chen, J. P. 2007. Methamphetamine-associated acute
    myocardial infarction and cardiogenic shock with normal
    coronary arteries: refractory global coronary microvascular
    spasm. J. Invasive Cardiol. 19:E89ÔÇôE92.
    Clatworthy, P. L., S. J. Lewis, L. Brichard, Y. T. Hong, D.
    Izquierdo, L. Clark, et al. 2009. Dopamine release in
    dissociable striatal subregions predicts the different effects of
    oral methylphenidate on reversal learning and spatial
    working memory. J. Neurosci. 29:4690ÔÇô4696.
    Cooper, N. J., H. Keage, D. Hermens, L. M. Williams,
    D. Debrota, and C. R. Clark. 2005. The dose-dependent
    effect of methylphenidate on performance, cognition,
    and psychophysiology. J. Integrative Neurosci. 4:
    123ÔÇô144.
    Cooper, W. O., L. A. Habel, C. M. Sox, K. A. Chan, P. G.
    Arbogast, T. C. Cheetham, et al. 2011. ADHD drugs and
    serious cardiovascular events in children and young adults.
    N. Engl. J. Med. 365:1896ÔÇô1904.
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 673
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDDepartment of Justice: Drug Enforcement Administration.
    2008. ARCOS2-report 7, United States summary for retail
    drug purchases by grams weight.
    Dodds, C. M., U. Muller, L. Clark, A. van Loon, R. Cools, and
    T. W. Robbins. 2008. Methylphenidate has differential
    effects on blood oxygenation level-dependent signal related
    to cognitive subprocesses of reversal learning. J. Neurosci.
    28:5976ÔÇô5982.
    Dopheide, J. A., and S. R. Pliszka. 2009. Attention-de´¼ücit-
    hyperactivity disorder: an update. Pharmacotherapy 29:
    656ÔÇô679.
    Elia, J., P. J. Ambrosini, and J. L. Rapoport. 1999. Treatment
    of attention-de´¼ücit-hyperactivity disorder. N. Engl. J. Med.
    340:780ÔÇô788.
    Elliott, R., B. J. Sahakian, K. Matthews, A. Bannerjea, J.
    Rimmer, and T. W. Robbins. 1997. Effects of
    methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning
    in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology 131:196ÔÇô206.
    European Medicines Agency. 2007. Meeting highlights from
    the committee for medicinal products for human use, 16ÔÇô19
    July 2007, European Medicines Agency, London.
    Farah, M. J., C. Halmm, G. Sankoorikal, M. E. Smith, and A.
    Chatterjee. 2009. When we enhance cognition with Adderall,
    do we sacri´¼üce creativity? A preliminary study
    Psychopharmacology 202:541ÔÇô547.
    Faraone, S. V., and S. J. Glatt. 2010. A comparison of the
    ef´¼ücacy of medications for adult attention-de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder using meta-analysis of effect sizes. J.
    Clin. Psychiatry 71:754ÔÇô763.
    Faraone, S. V., J. Biederman, J. G. Jetton, and M. T. Tsuang.
    1997. Attention de´¼ücit disorder and conduct disorder:
    longitudinal evidence for a familial subtype. Psychol. Med.
    27:291ÔÇô300.
    Faraone, S. V., J. Sergeant, C. Gillberg, and J. Biederman.
    2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is it an
    American condition? World Psychiatry 2:104ÔÇô113.
    Fillmore, M. T., T. H. Kelly, and C. A. Martin. 2005. Effects
    of d-amphetamine in human models of information
    processing and inhibitory control. Drug Alcohol Dep.
    77:151ÔÇô159.
    Fitzpatrick, P., R. Klorman, J. T. Brumaghim, and R. W.
    Keefover. 1988. Effects of methylphenidate on stimulus
    evaluation and response processes: evidence from
    performance and event-related potentials. Psychophysiology
    25:292ÔÇô304.
    Fleming, K., L. B. Bigelow, D. R. Weinberger, and T. E.
    Goldberg. 1995. Neuropsychological effects of amphetamine
    may correlate with personality characteristics.
    Psychopharmacol. Bull. 31:357ÔÇô362.
    Gandhi, P. J., G. U. Ezeala, T. T. Luyen, T. C. Tu, and M. T.
    Tran. 2005. Myocardial infarction in an adolescent taking
    Adderall. Am. J. Health Syst. Pharm. 62:1494ÔÇô1497.
    Greydanus, D. E., and V. C. Strasburger. 2006. Adolescent
    medicine. Prim. Care 33:xiiiÔÇôxviii.
    Gualtieri, C. T., and L. G. Johnson. 2008. Medications do not
    necessarily normalize cognition in ADHD patients. J. Atten.
    Disord. 11:459ÔÇô469.
    Habel, L. A., W. O. Cooper, C. M. Sox, K. A. Chan, B. H.
    Fireman, P. G. Arbogast, et al. 2011. ADHD medications
    and risk of serious cardiovascular events in young and
    middle-aged adults. JAMA 306:2673ÔÇô2683.
    Harris, S.. 2009. ÔÇ£I donÔÇÖt see it as a problemÔÇØ: more medical
    students taking prescription stimulants, few see cause for
    concern.
    Herman, L., O. Shtayermman, B. Aksnes, M. Anzalone, A.
    Cormerals, and C. Liodice. 2011. The use of prescription
    stimulants to enhance academic performance among college
    students in health care programs. J. Physician Assist. Educ.
    22:15ÔÇô22.
    Hester, R., L. S. Nandam, R. G. OÔÇÖConnell, J. Wagner, M.
    Strudwick, P. J. Nathan, et al. 2012. Neurochemical
    enhancement of conscious error awareness. J. Neurosci.
    32:2619ÔÇô2627.
    Horrigan, J. P. 2001. Present and future pharacotherapeutic
    options for adult attention de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder.
    Exp. Opin. Psychopharmacol. 2:573ÔÇô586.
    Hurst, P. M., R. Radlow, N. C. Chubb, and S. K. Bagley. 1969.
    Effects of D-amphetamine on acquisition, persistence, and
    recall. Am. J. Psychol. 82:307ÔÇô319.
    Jacobs, A. 2005. The Adderall advantage. The New York Times.
    Available at: http://www.nytimes.com/2005/07/31/education/
    edlife/jacobs31.html
    Jensen, P. S., L. E. Arnold, J. M. Swanson, B. Vitiello, H. B.
    Abikoff, L. L. Greenhill, et al. 2007. 3-year follow-up of the
    NIMH MTA study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry
    46:989ÔÇô1002.
    Jiao, X., S. Velez, J. Ringstad, V. Eyma, D. Miller, and M.
    Bleiberg. 2009. Myocardial infarction associated with
    Adderall XR and alcohol use in a young man. J. Am. Board
    Fam. Med. 22:197ÔÇô201.
    Johnston, L. D., P. M. OÔÇÖMalley, J. G. Bachman, and J. E.
    Schulenberg. 2004. Monitoring the future: national survey
    results on drug use, 1975ÔÇô2003. Pp. 10ÔÇô12 in NIoD, ed.
    Abuse. National Institutes of Health, U.S. Department of
    Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD.
    Kadison, R. 2005. Getting an edge ÔÇô use of stimulants and
    antidepressants in college. N. Engl. J. Med. 353:1089ÔÇô1091.
    Kennedy, R. S., R. C. Odenheimer, D. R. Baltzley, W. P. Dunlap,
    and C. D. Wood. 1990. Differential effects of scopolamine
    and amphetamine on microcomputer-based performance
    tests. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 61:615ÔÇô621.
    Kessler, R. C., L. Adler, R. Barkley, J. Biederman, C. K.
    Conners, O. Demler, et al. 2006. The prevalence and
    correlates of adult ADHD in the United Stated: results from
    the national comorbidity survey replication. Am. J.
    Psychiatry 163:716ÔÇô723.
    Klorman, R., L. O. Bauer, H. W. Coons, J. L. Lewis, J.
    Peloquin, R. A. Perlmutter, et al. 1984. Enhancing effects of
    674 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessnermethylphenidate on normal young adultsÔÇÖ cognitive
    processes. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 20:3ÔÇô9.
    Koelega, H. S. 1993. Stimulant drugs and vigilance
    performance: a review. Psychopharmacology 111:1ÔÇô16.
    Kollins, S. H. 2003. Comparing the abuse potential of
    methylphenidate versus other stimulants: a review of
    available evidence and relevance to the ADHD patient.
    J. Clin. Psychol. 64:14ÔÇô18.
    Kollins, S. H. 2008. ADHD, substance use disorder, and
    psychostimulant treatment current literature and treatment
    guidelines. J. Atten. Disord. 12:115ÔÇô125.
    Kollins, S. H., F. J. McClernon, and B. F. Fuemmeler. 2005.
    Association between smoking and attention-de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a population-based
    sample of children aged 9 through 16 years. J. Am. Acad.
    Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 62:1142ÔÇô1431.
    Kuczenski, R., and D. S. Segal. 1997. Effects of
    methylphenidate on extracellular dopamine, serotonin, and
    norepinephrine: comparison with amphetamine.
    J. Neurochem. 68:2032ÔÇô2037.
    Kumari, V., P. J. Corr, O. F. Mulligan, P. A. Cotter, S. A.
    Checkley, and I. A. Gray. 1997. Effects of acute
    administration of d-amphetamine and haloperidol on
    procedural learning in man: a preliminary study.
    Psychopharmacology 129:271ÔÇô276.
    Kutcher, J. S. 2011. Treatment of attention-de´¼ücit hyperactivity
    disorder in athletes. Curr. Sports Med. Rep. 10:32ÔÇô36.
    Looby, A., and M. Earleywine. 2011. Expectation to receive
    methylphenidate enhances subjective arousal but not
    cognitive performance. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 19:433ÔÇô
    444.
    Lord, S., G. Downs, P. Furlaw, A. Chaudhurl, A. Silverstein, A.
    Gammaitoni, et al. 2003. Nonmedical use of prescription
    opiods and stimulants among student pharmacists. J. Am.
    Pharm. Assoc. 49:519ÔÇô528.
    Low, K. G., and A. E. Gendaszek. 2002. Illicit use of
    psychostimulants among college students: a preliminary
    study. Psychol. Health Med. 7:283ÔÇô287.
    Makris, A. P., C. R. Rush, R. C. Frederich, A. C. Taylor, and
    T. H. Kelly. 2007. Behavioral and subjective effects of d-
    ampetamine and moda´¼ünil in healthy adults. Exp. Clin.
    Psychopharmacol. 15:123ÔÇô133.
    Masand, P., P. Pickett, and G. B. Murray. 1991.
    Psychostimulants for secondary depression in medical
    illness. Psychosomatics 32:203ÔÇô208.
    Mattay, V. S., K. F. Berman, J. L. Ostrem, G. Esposito, J. D. Van
    Horn, and L. B. Bigelow. 1996. Dextroamphetamine enhances
    ÔÇ£neural network-speci´¼ücÔÇØ physiological signals: a positron
    emission tomography rCBF study. J. Neurosci. 16:
    4816ÔÇô4822.
    Mattay, V. S., J. H. Callicott, A. Bertolino, I. Heaton, J. A.
    Frank, R. Coppola, et al. 2000. Effects of
    dextroamphetamine on cognitive performance and cortical
    activation. NeuroImage 12:268ÔÇô275.
    Mattay, V. S., T. E. Goldberg, F. Fera, A. R. Hariri, A.
    Tessitore, M. F. Egan, et al. 2003. Catechol O-
    methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual
    variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc. Natl.
    Am. Sci. USA 100:6186ÔÇô6191.
    McCabe, S. E., and C. J. Boyd. 2005. Sources of prescription
    drugs for illicit use. Addict. Behav. 30:1342ÔÇô1350.
    McCabe, S. E., C. J. Teter, and C. J. Boyd. 2004. The use,
    misuse and diversion of prescription stimulants among
    middle and high school students. Subst. Use Misuse
    39:1095ÔÇô1116.
    McCabe, S. E., J. R. Knight, C. J. Teter, and H. Wechsler.
    2005. Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants among US
    college students: prevalence and correlates from a
    nationwide survey. Addiction 100:96ÔÇô106.
    McLaughlin, L. 2011. Adderall: the whole story. Available at:
    http://reesenews.org/2011/10/26/adderall-the-whole-story/
    19843/
    McNiel, A. D., K. B. Muzzin, J. P. DeWald, A. L. McCann, E.
    D. Schneiderman, J. Sco´¼üeld, et al. 2011. The nonmedical
    use of prescription stimulants among dental and dental
    hygiene students. J. Dental Edu. 75:365ÔÇô376.
    Mehta, M. A., A. M. Owen, B. J. Sahakian, N. Mavaddat, J. D.
    Pickard, and T. W. Robbins. 2000. Methylphenidate enhances
    working memory by modulating discrete frontal and parietal
    lobe regions in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 20:RC65.
    Mintzer, M. Z., and R. R. Grif´¼üths. 2007. A triazolam/
    amphetamine dose-effect interaction study: dissociation of
    effects on memory versus arousal. Psychopharmacology
    192:425ÔÇô440.
    Misener, V. L., P. Luca, O. Azeke, J. Crosbie, I. Waldman, R.
    Tannock, et al. 2004. Linkage of the dopamine receptor D1
    gene to attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder. Mol.
    Psychiatry 9:500ÔÇô509.
    Mitchell, H.. 2012. Faking ADHD Gets you into Harvard. The
    Daily Beast. Available at: http://www.thedailybeast.com/
    articles/2012/01/25/faking-adhd-gets-you-into-harvard.html
    Molina, B. S. G., S. P. Hinshaw, J. M. Swanson, L. E. Arnold,
    B. Vitiello, P. S. Jensen, et al. 2009. The MTA at 8 years:
    prospective follow-up of children treated for combined type
    ADHD in the multisite study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
    Psychiatry. 48:484ÔÇô500.
    Murray, J. B. 1998. Psychophysiological aspects of amphetamine-
    methamphetamine abuse. J. Psychol. 132:227ÔÇô237.
    Musser, C. J., P. A. Ahmann, F. W. Theye, P. Mundt, S. K.
    Broste, and N. Mueller-Rizner. 1998. Stimulant use and the
    potential for abuse in Wisconsin as reported by school
    administrators and longitudinally followed children. J. Dev.
    Behav. Pediatr. 19:187ÔÇô192.
    Oken, B. S., S. S. Kishiyama, and M. C. Salinsky. 1995.
    Pharmacologically induced changes in arousal: effects on
    behavioral and electrophysiologic measures of alertness
    and attention. Electroenceph Clin. Neurophysiol. 95:
    359ÔÇô371.
    ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 675
    S. E. Lakhan & A. Kirchgessner Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHDPartridge, B. J., S. K. Bell, J. C. Lucke, S. Yeates, and W. D.
    Hall. 2011. Smart drugs ÔÇ£as common as coffeeÔÇØ: media hype
    about neuroenhancement. PLoS One 6:e28416.
    Pelham W. E., Jr, K. McBurnett, G. W. Harper, R. Milich, D.
    A. Murphy, J. Clinton, et al. 1990. Methylphenidate and
    baseball playing in ADHD children: whoÔÇÖs on ´¼ürst?
    J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 58:130ÔÇô133.
    Piper, B. J., J. B. Fraiman, and J. S. Meyer. 2005. Repeated
    MDMA (ÔÇ£EcstasyÔÇØ) exposure in adolescent male rats alters
    temperature regulation, spontaneous motor activity,
    attention, and serotonin transporter binding. Dev.
    Psychobiol. 47:145ÔÇô157.
    Polanczyk, G., M. S. de Lima, B. L. Horta, J. Biederman, and
    L. A. Rohde. 2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a
    systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am. J.
    Psychiatry 164:942ÔÇô948.
    Polchert, S. E., and R. M. Morse. 1985. Pemoline abuse. JAMA
    254:946ÔÇô947.
    Putukian, M., J. B. Kreher, D. B. Coppell, J. L. Glazer, D. B.
    McKeag, and R. D. White. 2011. Attention de´¼ücit
    hyperactivity disorder and the athlete: an American Medical
    Society for Sports Medicine position statement. Clin. J.
    Sport Med. 21:392ÔÇô401.
    Rabiner, D. L., A. D. Anastopoulos, E. J. Costello, R. H.
    Hoyle, S. E. McCabe, and H. S. Swartzweder. 2009. The
    misuse and diversion of prescribed ADHD medications by
    college students. J. Atten. Disord. 13:144ÔÇô153.
    Rapoport, J. L., M. S. Buchsbaum, H. Weingartner, T. P.
    Zahn, C. Ludlow, and E. J. Mikkelsen. 1978.
    Dextroamphetamine: cognitive and behavioral effects in
    normal and prepubertal boys. Science 199:560ÔÇô563.
    Rasmussen, P., and C. Gillberg. 2000. Natural outcome of
    ADHD with developmental coordination disorder at age
    22 years: a controlled, longitudinal, community-based
    study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:1424ÔÇô
    1431.
    Rogers, R. D., A. J. Blackshaw, H. C. Middleton, K. Matthews,
    K. Hawtin, C. Crowley, et al. 1999. Tryptophan depletion
    impairs stimulus-reward learning while methylphenidate
    disrupts attentional control in healthy young adults:
    implications for the monoaminergic basis of impulsive
    behavior. Psychopharmacology 146:482ÔÇô491.
    Rosa-Neto, P., H. C. Lou, P. Cumming, O. Pryds, H.
    Karrebaek, J. Lunding, et al. 2005. Methylphenidate-evoked
    changes in striatal dopamine correlate with inattention and
    impulsivity in adolescents with attention de´¼ücit
    hyperactivity/disorder. Neuroimage 25:868ÔÇô876.
    Rosenthal, E. 2012. Not acute myocardial infarction in a
    teenager due to Adderall XR. Pediatr. Cardiol. 33:679.
    Sahakian, B. J., and A. M. Owen. 1992. Computerized
    assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion
    paper. J. Royal Soc. Med. 85:399ÔÇô402.
    Schelleman, H., W. B. Bilker, S. E. Kimmel, G. W. Daniel, C.
    Newcomb, J. P. Guevara, et al. 2012. Methylphenidate and
    risk of serious cardiovascular events in adults. Am. J.
    Psychiatry 169:178ÔÇô185.
    Schmedje, J. F., C. M. Oman, R. Letz, and E. L. Baker. 1988.
    Effects of scopolamine and dextroamphetamine on human
    performance. Aviation Space Environ. Med. 59:407ÔÇô410.
    Schroeder, S. R., K. Mann-Koepke, C. T. Gualtieri, D. A.
    Eckerman, and G. R. Breese. 1987. Methylphenidate affects
    strategic choice behavior in normal adult humans.
    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 28:213ÔÇô217.
    Schweitzer, J. B., D. O. Lee, R. B. Hanford, C. F. Zink, T. D.
    Ely, M. A. Tagamets, et al. 2004. Effect of methylphenidate
    on executive functioning in adults with attention de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder: normalization of behavior but not
    related brain activity. Biol. Psychiatry 56:597ÔÇô606.
    Sciutto, M. J., C. J. Nol´¼ü, and C. Bluhm. 2004. Effects of child
    gender and symptom type on referrals for ADHD by
    elementary school teachers. J. Emotion Behav. Disord.
    12:247ÔÇô253.
    Servan-Schreiber, D., C. S. Carter, R. M. Bruno, and J. D.
    Cohen. 1998. Dopamine and the mechanisms of cognition:
    Part II. D-amphetamine effects in human subjects
    performing a selective attention task. Biol. Psychiatry
    43:723ÔÇô729.
    Silber, B. Y., R. J. Croft, K. Papafotiou, and C. Stough. 2006.
    The acute effects of d-amphetamine and methamphtetamine
    on attention and psychomotor performance.
    Psychopharmacology 187:154ÔÇô169.
    Smith, M. E., and M. J. Farah. 2011. Are prescription
    stimulants ÔÇ£smart pillsÔÇØ? The epidemiology and cognitive
    neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal
    healthy individuals. Psychol. Bull. 137:717ÔÇô741.
    Soetens, E., R. Dhooge, and J. Hueting. 1993. Amphetamine
    enhances human-memory consolidation. Neurosci. Lett.
    161:9ÔÇô12.
    Soetens, E., S. Casaer, R. Dhooge, and J. Hueting. 1995. Effect
    of amphetamine on long-term retention of verbal material.
    Psychopharmacology 119:155ÔÇô162.
    Sroufe, L. A. 2012. Ritalin gone wrong. New York Times, 29
    January 2012.
    Strauss, J., J. L. Lewis, R. Klorman, L. J. Peloquin, R. A.
    Perlmatter, and L. F. Salzman. 1984. Effects of
    methylphenidate on young adults performance and event-
    related potentials in a vigilance and a paired-associates
    learning test. Psychophysiology 21:609ÔÇô621.
    Surles, L. K., H. J. May, and J. P. Garry. 2002. Adderall-
    induced psychosis in an adolescent. J. Am. Board Fam.
    Pract. 15:498ÔÇô500.
    Sylvester, A. L., and B. Agarwala. 2012. Acute myocardial
    infarction in a teenager due to Adderall XR. Pediatr.
    Cardiol. 33:155ÔÇô157.
    Teter, C. J., S. E. McCabe, J. A. Cranford, C. J. Boyd, and S.
    K. Guthrie. 2005. Prevalence and motivations for the illicit
    use of prescription stimulants in an undergraduate student
    sample. J. Am. Coll. Health 53:253ÔÇô262.
    676 ?¬ 2012 The Authors. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
    Stimulants in Individuals With and Without ADHD S. E. Lakhan & A. KirchgessnerThe MTA Cooperative Group. 1999. A 14-month randomized
    clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56:1073ÔÇô1086.
    Tuttle, J. P., N. E. Scheurich, and J. Ranseen. 2010. Prevalence
    of ADHD diagnosis and nonmedical prescription stimulant
    use in medical students. Acad. Psychiatry 34:220ÔÇô223.
    Unrug, A., A. Coenen, and G. van Luijtelaar. 1997. Effects of
    the tranquilizer diazepam and the stimulant
    methylphenidate on alertness and memory.
    Neuropsychobiology 36:42ÔÇô48.
    Upadhyaya, H. P., K. Rose, W. Wang, K. OÔÇÖRourke, B.
    Sullivan, D. Deas, et al. 2005. Attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity
    disorder, medication treatment, and substance use patterns
    among adolescents and young adults. J. Child Adolesc.
    Psychopharmacol. 15:799ÔÇô809.
    Visser, S. N., C. A. Lesesne, and R. Perou. 2012. National
    estimates and factors associated with medication treatment
    for childhood attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder.
    Pediatrics 119:S99ÔÇôS107.
    Vitiello, B., G. R. Elliott, J. M. Swanson, L. E. Arnold, L.
    Hechtman, H. Abikoff, et al. 2011. Blood pressure and heart
    rate in the mulimodal treatment of attention de´¼ücit/
    hyperactivity disorder study over 10 years. Am. J.
    Psychiatry. 169:167ÔÇô177.
    Volkow, N. D., Y. S. Ding, J. S. Fowler, G. J. Wang, J. Logan,
    J. S. Gatley, et al. 1995. Is methylphenidate like cocaine?
    Studies on their pharmacokinetics and distribution in the
    human brain. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52:456ÔÇô463.
    Volkow, N. D., G. J. Wang, J. Newcorn, F. Telang, M. V.
    Solanto, J. S. Fowler, et al. 2007. Depressed dopamine
    activity in caudate and preliminary evidence of limbic
    involvement in adults with attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity
    disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64:932ÔÇô940.
    Volkow, N. D., G. J. Wang, D. Tomasi, S. H. Kollins, T. L.
    Wigal, J. H. Newcorn, et al. 2012. Methylphenidate-
    elicited dopamine increases in ventral striatum are
    associated with long-term symptom improvement in
    adults with attention de´¼ücit hyperactivity disorder.
    J. Neurosci. 32:841ÔÇô849.
    Ward, A. S., T. H. Kelly, R. W. Foltin, and M. W. Fischman.
    1997. Effects of d-amphetamine on task performance and
    social behavior of humans in a residential laboratory. Exp.
    Clin. Psychopharmacol. 5:130ÔÇô136.
    Weitzner, M. 1965. Manifest anxiety, amphetamine and
    performance. J. Psychol. 60:71ÔÇô79.
    Weyandt, L. L., and G. DuPaul. 2006. ADHD in college
    students. J. Atten. Disord. 10:9ÔÇô19.
    Wilens, T. E. 2003. Does the medicating ADHD increase or
    decrease the risk for later substance abuse? Rev. Bras.
    Psiquiatr. 25:127ÔÇô128.
    Wilens, T. E., J. Biederman, and T. J. Spencer. 2002. Attention
    de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Annu. Rev.
    Med. 53:113ÔÇô131.
    Wilens, T. E., S. Faraone, and J. Biederman. 2004.
    Attention-de´¼ücit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. JAMA
    292:619ÔÇô623.
    Wilens, T. E., L. A. Adler, J. Adams, S. Sgambati, J. Rotrosen,
    R. Sawtelle, et al. 2008. Misuse and diversion of stimulants
    prescribed for ADHD: a systematic review of the literature.
    J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47:21ÔÇô31.
    Willett, R. A. 1962. The effect of a stimulant and a depressant
    drug on the serial rote learning of nonsense syllables.
    Psychopharmacologia 3:23ÔÇô34.
    Wise, R. A. 2002. Brain reward circuitry: insights from
    unsensed incentives. Neuron 36:229ÔÇô240.
    de Wit, H., J. Crean, and J. B. Richards. 2000. Effects of
    d-amphetamine and ethanol on a measure of behavioral
    inhibition in humans. Behav. Neurosci. 114:830ÔÇô837.
    de Wit, H., J. L. Enggasser, and J. B. Richards. 2002. Acute
    administration of d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity
    in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:813ÔÇô
    825.
    Zeeuws, I., and E. Soetens. 2007. Verbal memory performance
    improved via an acute administration of D-amphetamine.
    Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 22:279ÔÇô287.
    Zuvekas, S., and B. Vitiello. 2011. Stimulant medication use in
    children: a 12-year perspective. Am. J. Psychiatry 169:160ÔÇô
    166